Neuron Article State-Dependent Variability of Neuronal Responses to Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Visual Cortex Brian N. Pasley,1,2,3 Elena A. Allen,1,2,3 and Ralph D. Freeman1,2,* 1Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute of Optometry University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA 3These authors contributed equally to this work *Correspondence: [email protected] DOI 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.012 2School SUMMARY Electrical brain stimulation is a promising tool for both experimental and clinical applications. However, the effects of stimulation on neuronal activity are highly variable and poorly understood. To investigate the basis of this variability, we performed extracellular recordings in the visual cortex following application of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Our measurements of spiking and local field potential activity exhibit two types of response patterns which are characterized by the presence or absence of spontaneous discharge following stimulation. This variability can be partially explained by state-dependent effects, in which higher pre-TMS activity predicts larger post-TMS responses. These results reveal the possibility that variability in the neural response to TMS can be exploited to optimize the effects of stimulation. It is conceivable that this feature could be utilized in real time during the treatment of clinical disorders. INTRODUCTION There is an extensive history of attempts to alter brain function using external electrical stimulation (Fritsch and Hitzig, 1870; Kringelbach et al., 2007). A primary focus of this work has been to establish neural modifications that relieve specific clinical disorders. Conditions such as Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, or depression, which often appear resistant to pharmacological intervention, have shown major improvement after treatment with invasive electrical stimulation techniques (Kringelbach et al., 2007). The success of these invasive interventions has generated interest in the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), a comparatively noninvasive technique (Barker et al., 1985). However, the effectiveness of TMS in therapeutic applications is not clear, and this emphasizes the need for a basic understanding of TMS mechanisms (Burt et al., 2002; Couturier, 2005; Fregni et al., 2005; George et al., 1996; Gross et al., 2007; Martin et al., 2003). The major challenge facing the therapeutic use of TMS, or any brain stimulation technique, is the difficulty in predicting how underlying neural circuits will be altered by the application of electrical fields. This problem is inherently complex, as the cumulative effect of stimulation depends on numerous factors. These may include: the structure of the targeted neural circuit, the profile of neural activity during application, the responses of different cell classes (e.g., excitatory versus inhibitory; projecting versus local neurons), the resulting biochemical or structural modifications of synaptic connections, and the possible alterations of neuromodulatory inputs. Combined with these biological factors are also a number of flexible stimulation parameters, such as duration, frequency, intensity, and electric field orientation. Each of these variables has been found previously to alter the outcome of TMS application (Berardelli et al., 1998; Chen et al., 1997; Pascual-Leone et al., 1998). Given the dependence of the effects of TMS on physiological state, brain region, and stimulation paradigms, it is difficult to identify general principles by which brain stimulation affects neural function. It is not surprising, therefore, that the literature in this field contains some contradictory and potentially confusing findings. For example, identical stimulation parameters can result in neuronal activation, suppression, or both, depending on the brain region (Paus, 2005). In addition, substantial intersubject variation has been noted both within healthy populations (Cahn et al., 2003) and with respect to patient populations (Brighina et al., 2002). Furthermore, even within the same individual, the effects of TMS appear to depend on the initial cortical activation state (for a review, see Silvanto and Muggleton, 2008). In these latter experiments, TMS produces different perceptual or behavioral outcomes that may depend on the excitability levels of specific neuronal populations (Silvanto and Muggleton, 2008). The apparent subtlety and complexity of the physiological effects of TMS necessitate empirical investigation in order to understand the stimulation-induced neural activity patterns. The shortage of available neural data describing the effects of TMS (e.g., see Allen et al., 2007; de Labra et al., 2007; Moliadze et al., 2003, 2005), coupled with a potentially broad use of TMS, motivates the investigation we describe here. We have conducted neurophysiological recordings of spiking activity and local field potentials (LFPs) in the visual cortex of anesthetized cats before, during, and after TMS application. A well-controlled Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 291 Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS study of TMS in an appropriate animal model is a necessary first step toward a basic understanding. In a previous report, we described primary neural responses to short TMS pulse trains and their relation to hemodynamic signals (Allen et al., 2007). In the current study, we undertake an extensive analysis to provide insight into the effects of TMS on single-neuron and population activity. We describe the variability of responses to TMS and find evidence for two qualitatively different response patterns which are characterized by the presence or lack of spontaneous discharge following stimulation. A portion of this variability can be explained by state-dependent effects, in which the postTMS response depends on pre-TMS activity levels. RESULTS We recorded single-unit and LFP responses at 47 sites in the primary visual cortex of the anesthetized cat (n = 5 animals). Single units were classified as simple (n = 17) or complex (n = 30), using the ratio of the first harmonic to the average firing rate (Skottun et al., 1991). Recordings were made with either posterior or superior positioning of a figure-eight TMS coil (Figure 1A). We find no significant differences in the neural responses to TMS between electrode-coil configurations of simple and complex cell classes (rank-sum test, p > 0.2 for all comparisons), and therefore the data are pooled for all analyses. Experimental Paradigm Each trial in our experimental paradigm (Figure 1B) consisted of a baseline period (40 s), application of a short TMS pulse train, and a post-TMS recovery period lasting from 5 to 15 min. TMS stimulation parameters were varied in frequency (1–8 Hz) and duration (1–4 s) on separate trials, with constant intensity at 100% stimulator output. Throughout each trial, a visual stimulus optimized to drive the cell was presented repeatedly for 2 s at 8 s intervals. As reported previously (Allen et al., 2007), we observe two primary effects of TMS. These include a transient elevation of spontaneous activity immediately following TMS, and a prolonged reduction in visually evoked activity that lasts for several minutes (Figure 1C). These different response components are seen clearly when the activity levels during and between presentations of visual stimuli are separated into evoked and spontaneous firing rates, respectively (Figure 1D). Additional experiments without interleaved visual stimuli showed comparable effects of TMS on spontaneous activity (see Figure S1 available online). Response Variability We analyzed the trial-by-trial variability of two TMS response components. The ‘‘spontaneous component’’ reflects the response to TMS itself. The ‘‘evoked component’’ reflects the effect of TMS on stimulus processing. Although the effects of TMS on these components are generally robust, we have observed considerable variability across both cells and trials. Figure 2 shows peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs) for four representative cells (A–D), each tested in two separate trials. These data represent the full range of response patterns we have observed and suggest an interesting distinction between 292 Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. Figure 1. TMS Coil Position and Experimental Paradigm (A) Illustration of the two coil-electrode configurations. At 28 sites in 3 cats, the coil was positioned posterior to the visual cortex and angled toward the horizontal plane (left). Penetrations were made with a carbon fiber electrode at an angle of P45, M0. At 19 recording sites in 2 cats, the coil was positioned obliquely near the transverse plane, superior to the visual cortex (right). Penetrations were made with a dual tungsten array (interelectrode spacing of 400 mm) at an angle of A45, M0. For both configurations, the midpoint of the coil was centered on the primary visual cortex craniotomy and was located between 1 and 2 cm from the skull. No significant differences between the neural responses to TMS were found for the different electrode-coil configurations (rank-sum test, p > 0.2), and thus these data were pooled in all analyses. (B) Timeline of a single trial. A visual stimulus (high-contrast drifting grating) was presented repeatedly for 2 s with an interstimulus interval of 8 s. After a baseline period (40 s), a short TMS pulse train (1–4 s, 2–8 Hz, 100% stimulator intensity) was applied during an interstimulus interval. Single-unit and LFP data were collected during response recovery (typically 5–15 min). (C) Peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) of spiking activity during a sample trial. Downward arrow at time zero denotes the application of a 4 Hz, 2 s TMS pulse train. In this and all subsequent PSTHs the bin size is 0.5 s. (D) Firing rate for the same trial as shown in (C), with activity separated into spontaneous and evoked components. The evoked response (dotted line) represents average activity during stimulus presentations, while the spontaneous component (solid line) indicates activity that occurred between stimuli. TMS response components: variability across trials appears greater for spontaneous compared to visually evoked responses. To quantify the variability of response components, we examined the relative standard deviation (RSD) of changes in spontaneous and evoked spiking activity in the first minute following TMS. This variability measure is similar to the Fano factor Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS Figure 3. Trend in Spontaneous Response to TMS over Time Figure 2. Examples of Variability in TMS Responses PSTHs of two sample trials with identical TMS parameters for four different cells. Downward solid arrows denote application of the TMS pulse train. Open arrows signify substantial spontaneous discharge following TMS. The stimulation parameters used in each example are as follows: (A) 4 Hz, 2 s; (B) 8 Hz, 4 s; (C) 4 Hz, 4 s; and (D) 4 Hz, 2 s. Evoked response components within single cells are more similar than those between cells. For example, some neurons reliably show moderate (D) or strong (B) reduction of evoked spiking following a TMS pulse train, whereas others consistently exhibit little alteration in stimulus-evoked activity (C). In contrast, spontaneous responses are extremely variable across identical trials within the same cell. In many instances (B–D), neurons display substantial spontaneous discharge on one trial but a complete absence of spontaneous firing on another. (Stevens and Zador, 1998), and accounts for differences in response amplitude by normalization of the standard deviation by the mean response (see Experimental Procedures). The RSD was calculated over trials with identical stimulation parameters at a given site (n = 23 sets of trials). Trial-to-trial variability in the spontaneous response (median RSD = 1.71) is significantly greater than that of the evoked (median RSD = 0.62, Wilcoxon signed-rank test paired by trial, p < 0.0005). We also compared the median RSD for trials within cells to the median RSD for equivalent trials across cells (see Experimental Procedures). For the evoked response, trial variability is significantly greater (A) PSTHs of seven consecutive trials from a single cell. A 4 Hz, 2 s TMS pulse train (downward arrow) was applied in each trial. PSTHs are truncated at 2 min to highlight spontaneous activity in the first 60 s following TMS (shaded area). Colors in (A) and (B) represent trial number. (B) Scatterplot of trial number versus the change in spontaneous firing rate (DRs) for the set of trials shown in (A). DRs is calculated as the difference between the average spike rate in the first minute following TMS and the average value during the baseline period. The dashed line indicates the least-squares fit to the data. (C) Scatterplot of normalized trial number versus normalized DRs for 23 sets of data (n = 112 total trials). For each set of data, the values for DRs and trial number were transformed into their respective ranks and then normalized by subtracting the mean rank. Symbols of different sizes are used to indicate the number of the trials at the same rank coordinates. Trial number and the spontaneous response exhibit a weak negative correlation (r = 0.26, p < 0.01, t test). No relationship is found between trial number and the evoked response (r = 0.07, p = 0.46, t test). across cells than within the same cell (permutation test, p < 0.01). The same is not true for the spontaneous response component (permutation test, p = 0.51). These results indicate not only greater trial-to-trial variability in spontaneous activity but also a lack of evidence for a characteristic spontaneous response to TMS that could distinguish one cell from another. Differences between spontaneous and evoked components are further evident when we examine trends in TMS responses over time. Throughout experiments, we observed that cells appeared more likely to exhibit spontaneous discharge on earlier trials. An example of this trend is shown in Figure 3A, which displays the PSTHs of seven consecutive trials from a single unit. Pronounced spontaneous spiking is evident in trials 1–4, but is considerably reduced or absent in trials 5–7 (Figures 3A and 3B). Analyzing all trials (grouped by cell and stimulation parameters), we find a weak, though significant, negative correlation between trial order and the magnitude of post-TMS Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 293 Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS spontaneous spiking (Figure 3C; r = 0.26, p < 0.01, t test). No similar relationship is found for evoked responses (r = 0.07, p = 0.46). A significant difference between spontaneous and evoked response trends (p < 0.01, one-tailed z test after Fisher’s transformation) argues against a simple decrease in TMS efficacy over time. Instead, these results suggest the presence of long-term or cumulative effects of TMS, which appear unique to spontaneous responses. The source of this long-term effect remains to be determined, but there is a suggestion of a sensitivity of the spontaneous response to baseline network properties (see below). Bursting versus Nonbursting Response Patterns The observation of seemingly all-or-none spontaneous responses motivated the division of trials into two qualitatively different groups, which we characterized as bursting (B) or nonbursting (NB). Trials in which the spontaneous firing rate in the first minute exceeded the baseline rate by two or more standard deviations were classified as B (n = 60/161). Trials showing a decrease or no change were classified as NB (n = 56/161). The remaining 45 trials exhibited an intermediate response (i.e., an increase of less than two standard deviations) and were not included in either group. Both B and NB trial types are observed in all animals and at virtually every recording site (100% when considering sites with at least four trials). There are no significant differences with regard to the proportion of trials at specific stimulation frequencies or durations (c2 test, p = 0.83 and p = 0.77, respectively). Additionally, simple and complex cell classes exhibit similar proportions of B and NB trials (c2 test, p = 0.71). Thus, the division of trials reflects the presence of distinct response patterns across trials, rather than across stimulation parameters or cells. To characterize the different responses of B and NB trials, we first examined the distributions of interspike intervals (ISIs) in each group. Figure 4 displays the logarithmic ISI histograms of spontaneous spikes for B (left) and NB (right) trials. The histograms of both response types are bimodal, with distinct peaks at short and long ISIs, a pattern frequently observed for cortical neurons (e.g., Reich et al., 2000). Prior to TMS (Figure 4A, top), the ISI peaks of B and NB trials are similarly located at roughly 3 and 200 ms (determined by fitting a mixture of Gaussians). Following TMS (Figure 4A, middle), the short ISI peak is unchanged for both trial types. ISIs of this length may reflect the small refractory period between action potentials (Izhikevich, 2006), suggesting that TMS does not alter this intrinsic cellular property. In contrast, TMS produces a substantial leftward shift in the long ISI peak of B trials, whereas the NB ISI distribution remains relatively unaltered. This shift is most prominent in the first 30 s post-TMS and there is a gradual recovery to baseline over 1–2 min (Figure 4B). The spontaneous discharge induced by TMS, therefore, appears to occur primarily at intervals of 20–40 ms, or 25–50 Hz. This frequency range corresponds to gamma band rhythms and is believed to involve activation of local sensory microcircuits, rather than a single cell (Liu and Newsome, 2006; Siegel and Konig, 2003). Interestingly, the disruption of spike intervals appears limited to spontaneous activity, as the ISI distributions of evoked spiking were relatively unaffected (see Figure S2). Differential responses of B and NB groups are also evident in the average time courses (Figure 5). By definition, B trials exhibit 294 Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. Figure 4. Distributions of Interspike Intervals before and after TMS (A) Log interspike interval (ISI) histograms of B trials (left) and NB trials (right) were constructed from spontaneous spikes (spikes occurring between presentation of visual stimuli) in 30 s windows. Each histogram spans from 0.4 ms to 8 s in 90 logarithmically spaced bins. Histograms are displayed for the 30 s prior to TMS (top), the 30 s immediately following TMS (middle), and a 30 s window occurring roughly 5 min after TMS. For all time periods, the histograms exhibit two separate ISI peaks, the locations of which are estimated by fitting a mixture of Gaussians. Superimposed over the histograms are the best-fit Gaussians for short (dark gray) and long (light gray) ISI peaks. (B) Locations of ISI peaks at short (squares) and long (circles) intervals for all time periods. Open symbols designate data for B trials, while filled symbols represent NB trials. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals, as estimated with a bootstrap resampling procedure (n = 1000 resamples) (Efron and Tibshirani, 1994). a large increase in spontaneous spiking, whereas NB trials show a small though significant and long-lasting reduction (Figures 5A and 5B). A similar response pattern for LFP power is evident in higher-frequency bands (30–150 Hz), where TMS induces an increase in B trials and a prolonged decrease in NB trials (Figures 5C and 5D). The similarity of LFP and spiking response patterns may appear trivial given the typically close association of these signals (Heeger and Ress, 2002). However, it is important to note that LFPs were classified based on single-unit spiking recorded at the same site. Because LFPs presumably reflect the aggregate activity of cells near the electrode tip (Logothetis et al., 2007; Mitzdorf, 1985), the differences in high-frequency LFP power suggest that neuronal responses to TMS can be relatively homogeneous within a local area (see also Spatial Correlation and Coherence section). Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS Figure 5. Response Time Courses for Bursting and Nonbursting Response Patterns (A) Average time courses of the change in spontaneous spiking activity from baseline (DRs) for B (open symbols) and NB trials (filled symbols). Error bars signify ± 1 SEM. (B) Average changes in DRs for time intervals I, II, and III, as denoted in (A). Intervals I, II, and III correspond roughly to the first, third, and fifth minute following TMS, respectively. Asterisks indicate a significant difference from baseline values (p < 0.05, sign-rank test, corrected). (C) Spectrograms showing the change in spontaneous LFP power (DLs) for B (top) and NB (bottom) trials. At each time point, DLs is calculated as a log ratio relative to the baseline spontaneous LFP power. Trials were classified as B or NB based on the activity of the single unit recorded at the same site. In these and subsequent spectrograms, data are color mapped symmetrically around zero such that positive values appear as warm colors, negative values appear as cool colors, and zero maps to green. (D) Average changes in DLs for time intervals I, II, and III as a function of different frequency bands. LFP bands, notated in (C), are defined as follows: d (delta; 1–4 Hz), q (theta; 4–8 Hz), a (alpha; 8–12 Hz), b (beta; 12–20 Hz), g (gamma; 20–80 Hz), hg (high gamma; 80–150 Hz). (E–H) Average time courses of changes in evoked spiking (E and F) and evoked LFP power (G and H), displayed in the same format as (A)–(D). Note that in (E), spontaneous activity directly preceding the presentation of a visual stimulus has been subtracted from the evoked response (see Experimental Procedures). In (H), a plus sign indicates a significant difference between B and NB responses (high gamma band, p < 0.05, rank-sum test, corrected). This difference likely indicates ‘‘contamination’’ from spontaneous activity. Because spontaneous LFP activity is present throughout the evoked response, elevations in this activity result in a smaller evoked decrease for B trials. In the lower-frequency LFP bands, B and NB responses are quite similar. Both groups show strong decreases in power that persist for longer than 5 min after TMS application (Figure 5D, bottom rows). The distinction between responses in the low and high frequencies may be related to the different functional roles attributed to specific brain rhythms (Belitski et al., 2008; Logothetis, 2008). For example, theta band activity is hypothesized to coor- dinate activity across distant cortical areas (Canolty et al., 2006), whereas gamma activity is thought to represent the synchronous processing of local neurons (Engel et al., 2001; Liu and Newsome, 2006). We next examined differences in evoked responses between B and NB groups. One might expect the presence or absence of strong spontaneous discharge to affect TMS-induced changes in stimulus-evoked activity. For example, strong discharge could fatigue the cells, resulting in a more pronounced reduction in evoked responses. Conversely, spontaneous discharge could signify strong activation of a local neural circuit which might facilitate evoked activity and produce a more moderate decrease, or even increase, in the stimulus-evoked response. The average time courses of evoked spiking, however, support neither of these scenarios. As shown in Figure 5E, the singleunit responses of B and NB trials are essentially identical. The effect of TMS on evoked LFPs is largely similar to that for spikes, in that both B and NB groups show decreases in power across nearly all frequencies (Figures 5G and 5H). The similar time courses of evoked activity for B and NB trials (Figures 5E–5H) contrast sharply with the dissimilar response Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 295 Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS pattern for spontaneous activity (Figures 5A–5D). It therefore appears that spontaneous and evoked response components are not inherently interrelated. The lack of correlation between changes in spontaneous and evoked spiking also supports this notion (r = 0.042, p > 0.5, t test, n = 161 trials). Figure 6. Influence of Baseline Variables on Responses to TMS (A) Distribution of stimulus-evoked responses (Re) during the baseline period for B (open, n = 60) and NB (filled, n = 56) trials. The average Re of B trials (mean ± SD: 35 ± 19 spikes/s, open arrow) is slightly greater than that of the NB trials (28 ± 17 spikes/s, filled arrow), leading to a significant difference between the distributions (p < 0.05, rank-sum test). (B) Scatterplot of baseline evoked activity (Re) and post-TMS spontaneous activity (Rs) for all trials (n = 161). Pre-TMS evoked activity and post-TMS spontaneous activity are significantly correlated (r = 0.30, p < 0.0001, t test). In this and subsequent panels, ‘‘post-TMS’’ variables are defined as the average value over the first minute following TMS (i.e., interval I). In addition, displayed correlations cannot be explained by differences in pre-TMS spontaneous activity, TMS stimulation parameters, or trial number, as factors potentially contributing explanatory power have been linearly regressed from both variables using partial correlation (see Experimental Procedures). (C) Scatterplot of pre-TMS evoked LFP high gamma power relative to spontaneous power (Le/s, hg; see Experimental Procedures) and post-TMS spontaneous spiking (Rs) for trials with single-unit and LFP data (n = 138). (D) Pearson correlation coefficients between baseline Le/s and post-TMS spontaneous spiking for all LFP frequency bands. The asterisk indicates a significant correlation (p < 0.05, t test, corrected). The arrow denotes the coefficient for the data displayed in (C). 296 Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. State-Dependent Effects Thus far, we have characterized the substantial variability of TMS-induced neural responses. We now investigate possible factors that may explain this variability. An intriguing possibility is that the effect of TMS in some way depends on the initial physiological state of the cortex. Numerous studies have noted robust differences when applying TMS during distinct brain states, for example during different levels of visual stimulation (Silvanto et al., 2007) or spatial attention (Bestmann et al., 2007). We have examined whether natural fluctuations in cortical activity could yield similar results by analyzing post-TMS responses as a function of preTMS activity levels. In these analyses, we use a partial correlation approach (see Experimental Procedures) which controls for the possible influence of additional factors. These factors include the mean amplitude of pre-TMS spontaneous activity, TMS stimulation parameters, and trial number. Therefore, reported correlations are those that remain after these factors have been linearly regressed from both pre- and post-TMS variables. One possible metric of cortical activity state is the responsiveness of cells to visual stimulation. We examined the distributions of pre-TMS evoked spiking responses for B and NB groups (Figure 6A). Although the distributions are broad and overlap considerably, trials classified as B are slightly more responsive to visual stimuli compared to those classified as NB. This difference is small, but significant (B: 35 ± 19 spikes/s, NB: 28 ± 17 spikes/s, mean ± SD; p < 0.05, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). A regression analysis including all trials (n = 161) indicates the same relationship: pre-TMS evoked spiking is positively correlated with TMS-induced spontaneous spiking (Figure 6B; r = 0.30, p < 0.0001). To examine visual responsiveness at the population level, we performed a similar regression analysis using pre-TMS stimulus-evoked LFPs. As shown in Figure 6C, the magnitude of (E) Power of baseline spontaneous LFPs as a function of trial type. Here, the LFP power in each band is relative to the total spectral power (see Experimental Procedures). Trials were classified as B or NB both by spiking activity (squares) and LFP power (circles). Single and double asterisks denote a significant difference between groups at p < 0.05 and p < 0.0005 criteria, respectively (rank-sum test, corrected). (F and G) Scatterplots of the relative baseline spontaneous LFP power and the post-TMS spontaneous LFP power (n = 142 trials). A significant positive correlation is found between baseline high gamma power and post-TMS high gamma power (F). A significant negative correlation is found between baseline alpha power and post-TMS beta power (G). (H) Correlation coefficients between the relative pre-TMS spontaneous power and the post-TMS spontaneous power for all frequency band combinations. To improve resolution beyond the six traditional bands (i.e., delta through high gamma), we divided the full frequency range (1–150 Hz) into 15 logarithmically spaced bins. The (ij)th element in the matrix corresponds to the correlation coefficient between the relative pre-TMS power in the ith frequency bin and the post-TMS Ls in the jth frequency bin. Elements outlined in black correspond to the data displayed in (F) and (G). Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS pre-TMS evoked high gamma power, relative to the spontaneous power in the same band, is significantly correlated with post-TMS spontaneous firing rate (r = 0.30, p < 0.0005, t test). Although a positive correlation is also observed for gamma band power, the lower-frequency bands instead exhibit negative correlations (Figure 6D). This finding is consistent with previous studies showing a suppression of low-frequency power during stimulus presentation and a general anticorrelation of power between lower and higher bands (Fries et al., 2001; Liu and Newsome, 2006; Niessing et al., 2005). Overall, these results indicate that strong cortical responsiveness to visual stimuli increases the likelihood of spontaneous discharge following TMS. A second possible metric of cortical activity state is the level of spontaneous, or ongoing, activity. Theoretically, both the baseline spontaneous spike rate and the baseline spontaneous LFP power can be used to independently assess cortical activity state. However, because cortical spontaneous spike rates are typically low (1.4 ± 1.8 spikes/s in this sample), they are not well suited for a correlation analysis. Thus, we focus our analysis on the relative LFP power during the pre-TMS period (see Experimental Procedures). The mean spontaneous LFP power spectra for B and NB groups are shown in Figure 6E. In this analysis, LFP trials were classified as NB or B using either post-TMS spontaneous spikes or post-TMS spontaneous LFP power. In both cases, trials were classified as B if TMS induced an increase of at least two standard deviations above baseline spontaneous activity, and as NB if there were a decrease or no change. Regardless of the classification scheme, B trials are associated with greater power in the high gamma band of pre-TMS spontaneous LFPs compared to NB trials (p < 0.05 for spikes-classifier, p < 0.0005 for LFP-classifier, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, corrected). At lower-frequency bands (theta and alpha), B trials have slightly less power than those classified as NB. Although this difference is difficult to see on the log scale of Figure 6E, it is statistically significant in the alpha band (p < 0.05 for LFP-classifier, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, corrected). To better understand the dependence of post-TMS spontaneous activity on baseline LFP power, we calculated the correlation coefficients between these variables for all pairs of frequency bands. This analysis results in a correlation matrix, shown in Figure 6H. Two general features are apparent in this matrix. First, correlations are positive at high frequencies of baseline LFP power, but negative for low frequencies. Examples of positive and negative correlations are shown in Figures 6F and 6G, respectively. Thus, greater relative power in the gamma and high gamma bands during the pre-TMS baseline predicts larger power in post-TMS spontaneous LFPs (e.g., Figure 6F). In contrast, greater relative baseline power in lower bands (delta to alpha) predicts smaller post-TMS power (e.g., Figure 6G). The change in correlation direction, which occurs in the lower beta band (15 Hz), demonstrates the general anticorrelation between low- and high-frequency power, as noted previously (Fries et al., 2001; Liu and Newsome, 2006; Niessing et al., 2005; Romei et al., 2008). A second important aspect of the correlation matrix is the presence of relatively stronger correlations at higher frequencies of the post-TMS spontaneous LFPs. Thus, pre-TMS sponta- neous LFP power is more predictive of post-TMS changes in high-frequency power than those at low frequency. This trend is not surprising, given that the increased variability associated with post-TMS spontaneous discharge appears primarily in the gamma and high gamma bands (Figure 5C). Taken together, these results suggest the following relationship. Application of TMS during a high activity state, as assessed with responsiveness to visual stimuli or the ongoing level of activity, is more likely to result in spontaneous discharge than application of the same pulse train during a low activity state. The above results describe relationships of state dependence between pre-TMS activity and post-TMS spontaneous activity. We have also performed similar analyses for post-TMS evoked activity. Changes in evoked activity show opposite trends compared to spontaneous activity: greater baseline spontaneous power in high-LFP bands (alpha and above) is associated with lower post-TMS evoked power (i.e., stronger reductions in the evoked activity). The direction of the association switches for lower bands of pre-TMS spontaneous LFPs, indicating negative correlations. The respective positive and negative correlations are present across all bands of the post-TMS evoked LFP power, although correlation coefficients are slightly greater in the higher bands. However, it should be noted that the magnitudes of these correlations are considerably weaker than those observed for post-TMS spontaneous activity and do not reach significance after correction for multiple comparisons. Spatial Correlation and Coherence In some experiments (n = 34 trials in 2 animals), we used a dualelectrode array to collect data simultaneously from two cortical sites spaced roughly 400 mm apart (Figure 7). These data permit us to ask whether neural activity in different cortical locations exhibits similar responses to TMS. In general, responses on the two electrodes are similar (Figure 7A), although there are differences with regard to response magnitude, particularly in high-frequency bands (Figure 7B). Interelectrode correlations consequently demonstrate a strong dependence on frequency band (Figure 7D). Changes in spontaneous LFPs (Figures 7C and 7D) are significantly correlated at low frequencies (delta through beta, r > = 0.44, p < 0.05, corrected), but not at higher frequencies. This trend is consistent with previous work demonstrating a stronger spatial coherence at lower frequencies (Destexhe et al., 1999). Evoked LFP responses reveal similar frequency dependence (Figure 7D), although overall correlations are weaker. This is likely due to the fact that visual stimuli were only optimized for neurons at one site, and did not reliably elicit neural responses on both electrodes. Thus, despite the spatially diffuse electric field produced by the TMS coil (Salinas et al., 2007), these interelectrode correlations indicate that the spontaneous response component is highly local in nature. Response homogeneity may be limited to a relatively small area (<400 mm). The simultaneous two-channel LFP data also allow us to investigate the effect of TMS on the timing of signals between different populations of neurons. Fine temporal relationships between the phases of neural signals have been associated with attention (Buschman and Miller, 2007; Fries et al., 2001; Saalmann et al., 2007), plasticity (Holscher et al., 1997; Wespatat et al., 2004), and memory (Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004), and are Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 297 Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS Figure 8. Effect of TMS on Spatial Coherence (A) Average levels of interelectrode LFP coherence (Cxy) during the pre-TMS baseline period for spontaneous (solid) and evoked (dotted) activity (n = 34 trials). Error bars signify ± 1 SEM. Asterisks indicate significantly greater coherence during evoked activity (sign-rank test, p < 0.05, corrected). (B) Spectrograms displaying the change in interelectrode coherence (DCxy) for spontaneous (top) and evoked (bottom) LFPs. DCxy is expressed as a percent change from baseline. (C) Average DCxy for different time intervals and frequency bands. Significant changes in spontaneous (left) and evoked (right) coherence are denoted with asterisks (p < 0.05, sign-rank test, corrected). Figure 7. Correlations between TMS Responses on Different Electrodes (A) Sample trace showing 8 s of spontaneous LFPs recorded from two different electrodes placed approximately 400 mm apart in area 17. Channel 1 denotes the electrode at which single-unit activity is isolated. (B) Example spectrograms from three different TMS trials showing changes in spontaneous LFP power (DLs) on channel 1 (left) and channel 2 (right). The TMS parameters used in each trial are as follows: sb331x1424, 8 Hz, 4 s; sb283x0701, 4 Hz, 4 s; and sb331x1003, 8 Hz, 4 s. (C) The changes in spontaneous theta band power (DLs, q) on channels 1 and 2 are significantly correlated (n = 34, p < 0.0001, t test). Here, DLs, q is calculated as the change in theta band power between the first minute post-TMS (interval I) and the pre-TMS baseline period. (D) Pearson correlation coefficients for DLs between channels 1 and 2 over all frequency bands. Asterisks indicate significant correlations (p < 0.05, t test, corrected). The arrow denotes the correlation coefficient for the data shown in (C). Note that possible confounds of these correlations (i.e., stimulation parameters and trial number) have been removed through partial correlation (see Experimental Procedures). often interpreted as indicators of functional ‘‘connectivity’’ between locations (Bruns, 2004; Lachaux et al., 1999; Pereda et al., 2005). Here we evaluated interelectrode phase synchrony using a common measure of spectral coherence. Because coherence is sensitive to both amplitude and phase relationships, we performed an additional interelectrode analysis exam- 298 Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. ining only phase-locking values (see Experimental Procedures). The results for these analyses are qualitatively similar, and we therefore describe results only for coherence. Figure 8A shows the baseline interelectrode coherence prior to TMS. The trend of coherence over different frequency bands and the significant elevation of high-frequency coherence during evoked responses (p < 0.005, corrected) are consistent with findings from previous studies (e.g., Henrie and Shapley, 2005). For TMS-induced responses, spontaneous LFPs (Figure 8B, top) at lower frequencies (8–20 Hz) exhibit a strong decrease in coherence that slowly decays (Figure 8C, left). At high frequencies (>80 Hz), we observe instead a slight increase in coherence (Figure 8C, left). Changes in evoked coherence (Figure 8B, bottom) are very similar, although evoked activity shows a more pronounced increase in high gamma coherence that persists for several minutes after TMS (Figure 8C, right). We note that the effects of TMS on interelectrode LFP-LFP spectral coherence and phase locking are similar to those found in our previous report on spike-LFP synchrony (Allen et al., 2007). The prior analysis examined the relationship between spike times and phases of the LFP oscillations recorded at the same electrode. Despite different types of data and methodology, both analyses indicate that TMS induces desynchronization and hypersynchronization at lower and higher frequencies, Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS respectively. These results demonstrate the capacity of TMS to alter signal timing between neural populations, and suggest that TMS may exert strong effects on functional processes that depend on spike timing or phase locking. DISCUSSION Our current study has evaluated the variability in neuronal responses following application of short TMS pulse trains during the resting state. We find evidence for two divergent response patterns, defined by the presence or absence of burst firing after stimulation. Importantly, this effect is shown to be state dependent: higher pre-TMS activity predicts greater post-TMS activity. Variability in the response to electrical stimulation is a wellknown phenomenon, observed both behaviorally (Ridding and Rothwell, 2007) and neurophysiologically (Kringelbach et al., 2007). In our data, variability is principally seen on a trial-to-trial basis in the degree of spontaneous burst firing. The effect of TMS on spontaneous activity is the focus of a considerable amount of TMS literature (e.g., Bestmann et al., 2008; Brighina et al., 2004; Hallett, 2007; Ridding and Rothwell, 2007; Romei et al., 2008; Sauseng et al., 2009; Silvanto et al., 2007; Van Der Werf et al., 2006). For example, TMS studies of phosphene or muscle twitch thresholds are frequently used to assess cortical excitability (Bestmann et al., 2007; Brighina et al., 2002; Hallett, 2007; Huang et al., 2005; Ridding and Rothwell, 2007; Stewart et al., 2001). These overt behavioral responses are thought to be analogs of TMS-induced spontaneous bursting. Stimulation-induced overt responses have been linked to direct activation of motor or sensory circuits (Tehovnik et al., 2006) and even single neurons (Houweling and Brecht, 2008; Huber et al., 2008). A hallmark of these threshold studies is the substantial trial-to-trial variability, in which overt responses are observed in some trials but not others. Our neurophysiological findings provide a close parallel to the robust variability noted in these behavioral studies. An additional important feature of threshold studies is that preexisting activity levels can modulate the stimulation intensity required to evoke an overt response. For example, motor or phosphene thresholds have been shown to be modulated by spatial attention (Bestmann et al., 2007), motor training (Butefisch et al., 2000), drug application (Oliveri and Calvo, 2003; Ziemann et al., 2002), epilepsy (Theodore, 2003), and migraine (Ambrosini et al., 2003). Our finding that the post-TMS burst response depends on pre-TMS activity levels is consistent with the hypothesis that changes in baseline activity levels underlie these behavioral modulations. Notably, recent studies have begun to investigate the cortical topography of such statedependent responses. Using concurrent TMS-fMRI, investigators have demonstrated that distinct activation patterns are produced depending on the behavioral task to which stimulation is paired (Ruff et al., 2006; Sack et al., 2007). In addition, the effect of TMS on spontaneous activity may be relevant to clinical applications. Clinical disorders are generally characterized by abnormal activity revealed during an ongoing state. The logic of TMS clinical treatment is that it causes disruption of ongoing activity of abnormal circuits (Hallett, 2007; Ridding and Rothwell, 2007). For example, electroconvulsive shock therapy utilized extensively for depression is thought to operate by this principle (Lisanby and Belmaker, 2000). Our finding that TMS disrupts the temporal structure of spatially remote sites is consistent with the hypothesis that TMS can be used to progressively alter abnormal neuronal communication. It is important to consider the circuit and cellular mechanisms that underlie the spontaneous response and associated statedependent effects. It is likely that TMS application directly induces activating current in a subset of cortical cells (Moliadze et al., 2003; Patton and Amassian, 1954). This activation can elicit reverberating excitatory potentials in postsynaptic cells, producing a persistent bursting response that outlasts the TMS pulse train (Patton and Amassian, 1954; Terao and Ugawa, 2002). As our data indicate, the spontaneous bursting response involves neural recruitment throughout the local microcircuit, and is therefore subject to the balance of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic activity. It is feasible that higher baseline excitability leads to recurrent excitation (i.e., bursting) upon application of the TMS pulse train, whereas lower baseline excitability signifies a relatively greater level of inhibition that dampens recurrent excitation and prevents burst firing. This explanation of state dependence is consistent with the current results and with those of numerous threshold studies (Bestmann et al., 2007; Butefisch et al., 2000; Oliveri and Calvo, 2003; Romei et al., 2008; Ziemann et al., 2002). In contrast to the state dependence observed for spontaneous activity, we found little evidence for state-dependent evoked activity. This may relate to different mechanisms underlying the spontaneous and evoked responses (see below). Weak evoked state dependence may also be due to the specifics of our stimulation paradigm. TMS was applied only during intervals of spontaneous activity, and therefore did not target a distinct neural population. This differs from a paradigm in which stimulation is applied during different tasks that recruit largely nonoverlapping neural populations (Silvanto and Muggleton, 2008). Previous behavioral work has demonstrated robust state-dependent effects when pairing stimulation to tasks with different profiles of neural activation (Silvanto and Muggleton, 2008). An improved understanding of how to exploit state-dependent effects could have important implications for optimizing stimulation procedures in therapeutic contexts (e.g., see Miller, 2007). Our results also permit an examination of a widely held conceptual account of how TMS interferes with neural function. This interference has been characterized as a ‘‘virtual lesion’’ (Pascual-Leone et al., 2000), in analogy to structural brain lesions that produce specific functional deficits. The large decrease in visually evoked activity following TMS supports this view, although the physiological processes underlying this suppression have yet to be established. One possible mechanism is long-term hyperpolarization, which may be due to alterations in extrinsic synaptic input or intrinsic membrane properties. For example, electrical stimulation has been shown to substantially elevate levels of extracellular GABA, which suppresses activity for several minutes (Mantovani et al., 2006). Alternatively, prolonged neuronal suppression might result from disruption of normally coordinated activity patterns at the circuit level. Our data and that of others (Jing and Takigawa, 2000; Oliviero et al., 2003; Strens et al., 2002) demonstrate that this coordination is disrupted by TMS. Specifically, the temporal relationships of neural signals, as measured by spike-LFP (Allen et al., 2007) Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 299 Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS and LFP-LFP phase synchrony (Figure 8), are altered for several minutes. If signal patterns between neurons are perturbed, one would expect a detrimental effect on the functions supported by those cells. Accordingly, when a neural circuit is probed with a visual stimulus following TMS, we find an immediate and prolonged reduction of evoked activity. The convergence of previous behavioral findings and the current neuronal analyses strongly suggests that variations in existing activity levels contribute to the variability of TMS responses. This relationship may explain, in part, the considerable discrepancies between subjects and trials found in many brain stimulation studies. Furthermore, our results suggest that the analysis of TMS responses in terms of the preceding activity may help to elucidate and interpret stimulation-induced response patterns. The direct monitoring of neural activity using noninvasive techniques, such as EEG (Massimini et al., 2005; Romei et al., 2008) or hemodynamic-based imaging (Allen et al., 2007; Bohning et al., 1999; Ruff et al., 2006; Sack et al., 2007), can empirically guide the effective use of TMS in both clinical and experimental settings. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES Animal Preparation All animal procedures are in compliance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and are approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California Berkeley. Mature cats (n = 5) are initially anesthetized with isofluorane (3%–4%). Following placement of venous catheters, isofluorane is discontinued, and anesthesia is maintained with intravenous infusion of fentanyl citrate (10 mg $ kg1 $ hr1) and thiopental sodium (initially 6.0 mg $ kg1 $ hr1). Following the placement of a tracheal cannula, animals are artificially ventilated with a 25% O2/75% N2O mixture. Respiration rate is adjusted to maintain expired CO2 between 30 and 36 mmHg (generally between 15 and 25 breaths/min). Body temperature is maintained at 38 C with a closed-loop controlled heating pad (Love Controls, IN, USA). A craniotomy over area 17 is performed (Horsley-Clarke coordinates P4, L2; Horsley and Clarke, 1908), and the dura resected. After completion of surgical procedures, fentanyl citrate infusion is discontinued, and the rate of thiopental sodium infusion is gradually lowered to a level at which the animal is stabilized (typically 1.5 mg $ kg1 $ hr1). After stabilization, paralysis is induced with pancuronium bromide (0.2 mg $ kg1 $ hr1) to prevent eye movements. EEG, ECG, heart rate, temperature, end-tidal CO2, and intratracheal pressure are monitored continuously throughout the duration of the experiment. Experimental Paradigm Visual stimuli (drifting sinusoidal gratings) are presented on a luminance-calibrated CRT monitor (85 Hz refresh rate, mean luminance 45 cd/m2). Preliminary tests are performed on each neuron to identify the stimulus orientation, spatial frequency, temporal frequency, position, and size to maximize the neuron’s spike response. During TMS trials, drifting gratings with optimal parameters are displayed at 50% contrast for 2 s. TMS is applied to the visual cortex using a Magstim Rapid system (Magstim Company, Whitland, UK) with a 70 mm figure-eight coil, which is positioned using a mechanical camera arm (see Figure 1A). Pulse trains are delivered by series of TTL digital pulses with parametrically varying frequency (1, 4, 8 Hz) and duration (1, 2, 4 s) at 100% stimulation intensity. At this intensity and range of distances (1–2 cm distance from the skull and an additional 3 mm between the skull and the cortical surface), the induced electric field strength is estimated to be 100–200 V/m (Salinas et al., 2007). To ensure neural recovery between TMS trials, each subsequent trial is initiated only when the evoked response has maintained a steady-state value for over 1 min. We include a minimum of 6 min between TMS applications, with typical intervals of 10–15 min. 300 Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. Data Collection Neural data are recorded using either NaCl-filled barrels from a multibarrel carbon fiber microelectrode (Kation Scientific, Minneapolis, MN, USA) or epoxy-coated tungsten microelectrodes (5 MU, A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA, USA). Tungsten electrodes are mounted in a dual array, allowing simultaneous recordings from spatially distinct regions (400 mm apart). For both electrode types, the LFP signal is obtained from the broadband neural trace by band-pass filtering between 0.7 and 170 Hz, and the data are digitized at 500 Hz. The multiunit signal is obtained from the broadband signal by filtering between 500 Hz and 8 MHz. Individual single units are discriminated online based on the temporal shapes of their extracellular potentials, and spike times are recorded with 0.04 ms precision. Single-unit data are included in the analysis only if the spike waveform remains stable throughout the duration of the TMS trial. Of the 47 single units in our sample, 45 have less than 0.1% of their ISIs within a typical refractory period of 1 ms. The other 2 cells exhibit a shorter (though not unusual; see Gur and Snodderly, 2006) refractory period and have less than 0.1% of events within 0.7 ms. Data Analysis TMS-induced electrical artifacts are removed from all analyses by excluding a window of data that spans from the first TMS pulse to 100 ms after the last pulse. Single-unit data are converted into spike rates (R) by dividing the number of spikes in a time window by the duration of that window. Spontaneous spike rate, Rs(t), is defined as the raw firing rate during each 8 s interstimulus interval. Evoked spike rate, Re(t), is defined as the average spike firing during each 2 s stimulus presentation following subtraction of the raw spontaneous rate that immediately precedes the stimulus. This subtraction assumes an additive model of spike generation, although it is important to note that none of our results were significantly altered by removing this subtraction from the analysis. The TMS-induced change in spontaneous spike rate, DRs, is defined as Rs(t) – Rs(tbaseline), where t denotes time and Rs(tbaseline) denotes the average spontaneous firing rate over the pre-TMS baseline period (40 s interval prior to TMS). The TMS-induced change in evoked spike rate, DRe, is defined analogously. Raw LFP signals are converted to LFP power (L) by first removing line noise at 60 and 85 Hz (monitor refresh rate), then using multitaper spectral estimation over 1 s windows and 5 Hz bandwidth (Pesaran et al., 2002; Thomson, 1982). The spontaneous LFP power, Lrs ðf; tÞ, is defined as the raw power in frequency band f during each spontaneous time interval. Evoked LFP power, Lre ðf; tÞ, is analogously defined for each interval of evoked activity. When comparing absolute values of LFP power, we used log transformations to normalize the data distributions (Cohen et al., 2003). Thus, Ls ðf; tÞ = logðLrs ðf; tÞÞ and Le ðf; tÞ = logðLre ðf; tÞÞ. Changes in LFP power can then be computed as the simple difference in transformed power values, for example, DLs ðfÞ = Ls ðf; tÞ Ls ðf; tbaseline Þ, which is mathematically equivalent to the log ratio of the raw power values: DLs ðfÞ = log Lrs ðf; tÞ Lrs ðf; tbaseline Þ : Similarly, the stimulus-evoked elevation in LFP power relative to the spontaneous activity immediately preceding stimulus (Figures 6C and 6D) can be defined as r L ðf; tbaseline Þ Le=s ðfÞ = log er Ls ðf; tbaseline Þ or Le=s ðfÞ = Le ðf; tbaseline Þ Ls ðf; tbaseline Þ. To effectively compare pre-TMS spontaneous LFPs from different sites (Figures 6E–6H), the spectral power of each trial is normalized by the area under the entire spectrum (Liu and Newsome, 2006). Thus, ‘‘relative pre-TMS Ls,’’ calculated as Lr ðf; tbaseline Þ Lrelative ðfÞ = P s r ; s Ls ðf; tbaseline Þ f refers to the relative power in each frequency band. To compare the variability of spontaneous and evoked responses, we compute the relative standard deviation (RSD) of each component for a given set of trials. Equivalent results were obtained using the Fano factor, which is Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS a standard measure of neuronal variability that accounts for differences in response amplitudes (Stevens and Zador, 1998). These measures are mathematically equivalent up to a square factor: RSD normalizes the standard deviation by the mean, whereas the Fano factor normalizes the square of the standard deviation. A set of trials is defined as three or more trials run under identical conditions (i.e., same site and stimulation parameters). Note that the same sets of trials (n = 23) are also used in the rank-correlation analysis (see Figure 3). Variability in response components is further evaluated by comparing trials recorded at a single cell to those recorded from different cells. This is achieved using a permutation test, resampling the population to form equivalent sets of trials with identical stimulation parameters but different sites. Significance is assessed by comparing the median RSD of the original sets of trials to the distribution of median RSDs from the resampled sets of trials (n = 10,000 resamples) (Manly, 1991). For correlation analyses including all trials (Figures 6 and 7), partial correlation is used to control for the possible influence of additional variables (Cohen et al., 2003). Pre- and post-TMS variables of interest are first regressed on confound factors that include stimulation parameters and trial number. In state-dependency analyses (Figure 6), the pre-TMS spontaneous activity (spike rate or LFP power, where appropriate) is included as an additional regressor. Correlation is then performed on the residuals. These residuals have the same units as the original variables, but have been linearly transformed. Thus, the pre- and post-TMS spike rate residuals can take on negative values (see Figure 6C). This partial correlation approach ensures that any observed relationship cannot be due to linear associations between additional variables. For synchrony analyses, LFP-LFP synchrony between recording sites is evaluated using the coherence statistic (Mitra and Pesaran, 1999): S ðfÞ xy Cxy ðfÞ = pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi; Sx ðfÞSy ðfÞ where Cxy is the coherence ranging from 0 to 1, f is frequency, Sx(f) and Sy(f) are the spectra of the signals recorded from the two sites, and Sxy(f) is the crossspectrum. Because coherence is a biased statistic which varies with sample size (Jarvis and Mitra, 2001), interelectrode coherence was always calculated over equivalent time windows (2 s duration). Because coherence is sensitive to both amplitude and phase coupling, we also computed a phase-locking value that is insensitive to amplitude changes (Lachaux et al., 1999; Pereda et al., 2005). The LFP signal was filtered in 5 Hz bands and the instantaneous phase at each time point was extracted via the Hilbert transform (Lachaux et al., 1999; Pereda et al., 2005). The phase-locking value was computed as pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi PLVðfÞ = jhei4ðtÞ ij, where f is frequency, 4ðtÞ is the difference between the phases at each electrode and at each time t, and h,i denotes the average over time (Lachaux et al., 1999; Pereda et al., 2005). The two synchrony measures were qualitatively similar and therefore results are reported for coherence only. SUPPLEMENTAL DATA Supplemental data include two figures and can be found with this article online at http://www.neuron.org/supplemental/S0896-6273(09)00211-6. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank R. Bartholomew, N. Lines, A. Koukarine, and L. Gibson for assistance in developing the electrophysiological apparatus and data acquisition software. This work was supported by research and CORE grants from the National Eye Institute (EY01175 and EY03176, respectively) and by NSF graduate research fellowship 2003014861. Accepted: March 6, 2009 Published: April 29, 2009 Ambrosini, A., de Noordhout, A.M., Sandor, P.S., and Schoenen, J. (2003). Electrophysiological studies in migraine: a comprehensive review of their interest and limitations. Cephalalgia 23 (Suppl 1), 13–31. Barker, A.T., Jalinous, R., and Freeston, I.L. (1985). Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Lancet 1, 1106–1107. Belitski, A., Gretton, A., Magri, C., Murayama, Y., Montemurro, M.A., Logothetis, N.K., and Panzeri, S. (2008). Low-frequency local field potentials and spikes in primary visual cortex convey independent visual information. J. Neurosci. 28, 5696–5709. Berardelli, A., Inghilleri, M., Rothwell, J.C., Romeo, S., Curra, A., Gilio, F., Modugno, N., and Manfredi, M. (1998). Facilitation of muscle evoked responses after repetitive cortical stimulation in man. Exp. Brain Res. 122, 79–84. Bestmann, S., Ruff, C.C., Blakemore, C., Driver, J., and Thilo, K.V. (2007). Spatial attention changes excitability of human visual cortex to direct stimulation. Curr. Biol. 17, 134–139. Bestmann, S., Swayne, O., Blankenburg, F., Ruff, C.C., Haggard, P., Weiskopf, N., Josephs, O., Driver, J., Rothwell, J.C., and Ward, N.S. (2008). Dorsal premotor cortex exerts state-dependent causal influences on activity in contralateral primary motor and dorsal premotor cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1281–1291. Bohning, D.E., Shastri, A., McConnell, K.A., Nahas, Z., Lorberbaum, J.P., Roberts, D.R., Teneback, C., Vincent, D.J., and George, M.S. (1999). A combined TMS/fMRI study of intensity-dependent TMS over motor cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 385–394. Brighina, F., Piazza, A., Daniele, O., and Fierro, B. (2002). Modulation of visual cortical excitability in migraine with aura: effects of 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 145, 177–181. Brighina, F., Piazza, A., Vitello, G., Aloisio, A., Palermo, A., Daniele, O., and Fierro, B. (2004). rTMS of the prefrontal cortex in the treatment of chronic migraine: a pilot study. J. Neurol. Sci. 227, 67–71. Bruns, A. (2004). Fourier-, Hilbert- and wavelet-based signal analysis: are they really different approaches? J. Neurosci. Methods 137, 321–332. Burt, T., Lisanby, S.H., and Sackeim, H.A. (2002). Neuropsychiatric applications of transcranial magnetic stimulation: a meta analysis. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 5, 73–103. Buschman, T.J., and Miller, E.K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. Butefisch, C.M., Davis, B.C., Wise, S.P., Sawaki, L., Kopylev, L., Classen, J., and Cohen, L.G. (2000). Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97, 3661–3665. Buzsaki, G., and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. Cahn, S.D., Herzog, A.G., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2003). Paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation: effects of hemispheric laterality, gender, and handedness in normal controls. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 20, 371–374. Canolty, R.T., Edwards, E., Dalal, S.S., Soltani, M., Nagarajan, S.S., Kirsch, H.E., Berger, M.S., Barbaro, N.M., and Knight, R.T. (2006). High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, 1626–1628. Chen, R., Classen, J., Gerloff, C., Celnik, P., Wassermann, E.M., Hallett, M., and Cohen, L.G. (1997). Depression of motor cortex excitability by lowfrequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 48, 1398–1403. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S.G., and Aiken, L.S. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum). REFERENCES Couturier, J.L. (2005). Efficacy of rapid-rate repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 30, 83–90. Allen, E.A., Pasley, B.N., Duong, T., and Freeman, R.D. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation elicits coupled neural and hemodynamic consequences. Science 317, 1918–1921. de Labra, C., Rivadulla, C., Grieve, K., Marino, J., Espinosa, N., and Cudeiro, J. (2007). Changes in visual responses in the feline dLGN: selective thalamic suppression induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation of V1. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1376–1385. Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 301 Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS Destexhe, A., Contreras, D., and Steriade, M. (1999). Spatiotemporal analysis of local field potentials and unit discharges in cat cerebral cortex during natural wake and sleep states. J. Neurosci. 19, 4595–4608. Logothetis, N.K., Kayser, C., and Oeltermann, A. (2007). In vivo measurement of cortical impedance spectrum in monkeys: implications for signal propagation. Neuron 55, 809–823. Efron, B., and Tibshirani, R. (1994). An Introduction to the Bootstrap (New York: Chapman & Hall). Manly, C.F.J. (1991). Randomization and Monte-Carlo Methods in Biology (New York: Chapman & Hall). Engel, A.K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. Mantovani, M., Van Velthoven, V., Fuellgraf, H., Feuerstein, T.J., and Moser, A. (2006). Neuronal electrical high frequency stimulation enhances GABA outflow from human neocortical slices. Neurochem. Int. 49, 347–350. Fregni, F., Simon, D.K., Wu, A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Non-invasive brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 1614–1623. Fries, P., Reynolds, J.H., Rorie, A.E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563. Fritsch, G., and Hitzig, E. (1870). Ueber die elektrishe Erregarkeit des Grosshirns (Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas). George, M.S., Wassermann, E.M., and Post, R.M. (1996). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a neuropsychiatric tool for the 21st century. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 8, 373–382. Gross, M., Nakamura, L., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fregni, F. (2007). Has repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) treatment for depression improved? A systematic review and meta-analysis comparing the recent vs. the earlier rTMS studies. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 116, 165–173. Martin, J.L., Barbanoj, M.J., Schlaepfer, T.E., Thompson, E., Perez, V., and Kulisevsky, J. (2003). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of depression. Systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 182, 480–491. Massimini, M., Ferrarelli, F., Huber, R., Esser, S.K., Singh, H., and Tononi, G. (2005). Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science 309, 2228–2232. Miller, G. (2007). Neuroscience. Uncovering the magic in magnetic brain stimulation. Science 317, 1846. Mitra, P.P., and Pesaran, B. (1999). Analysis of dynamic brain imaging data. Biophys. J. 76, 691–708. Mitzdorf, U. (1985). Current source-density method and application in cat cerebral cortex: investigation of evoked potentials and EEG phenomena. Physiol. Rev. 65, 37–100. Gur, M., and Snodderly, D.M. (2006). High response reliability of neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) of alert, trained monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 16, 888–895. Moliadze, V., Zhao, Y., Eysel, U., and Funke, K. (2003). Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on single-unit activity in the cat primary visual cortex. J. Physiol. 553, 665–679. Hallett, M. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a primer. Neuron 55, 187–199. Moliadze, V., Giannikopoulos, D., Eysel, U.T., and Funke, K. (2005). Paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation protocol applied to visual cortex of anaesthetized cat: effects on visually evoked single-unit activity. J. Physiol. 566, 955–965. Heeger, D.J., and Ress, D. (2002). What does fMRI tell us about neuronal activity? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 142–151. Henrie, J.A., and Shapley, R. (2005). LFP power spectra in V1 cortex: the graded effect of stimulus contrast. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 479–490. Holscher, C., Anwyl, R., and Rowan, M.J. (1997). Stimulation on the positive phase of hippocampal theta rhythm induces long-term potentiation that can be depotentiated by stimulation on the negative phase in area CA1 in vivo. J. Neurosci. 17, 6470–6477. Horsley, V., and Clarke, R. (1908). The structure and functions of the cerebellum examined by a new method. Brain 31, 45–124. Houweling, A.R., and Brecht, M. (2008). Behavioural report of single neuron stimulation in somatosensory cortex. Nature 451, 65–68. Huang, Y.Z., Edwards, M.J., Rounis, E., Bhatia, K.P., and Rothwell, J.C. (2005). Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45, 201–206. Huber, D., Petreanu, L., Ghitani, N., Ranade, S., Hromadka, T., Mainen, Z., and Svoboda, K. (2008). Sparse optical microstimulation in barrel cortex drives learned behaviour in freely moving mice. Nature 451, 61–64. Izhikevich, E.M. (2006). Bursting. Scholarpedia 1, 1300. Jarvis, M.R., and Mitra, P.P. (2001). Sampling properties of the spectrum and coherency of sequences of action potentials. Neural Comput. 13, 717–749. Jing, H., and Takigawa, M. (2000). Observation of EEG coherence after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1620–1631. Kringelbach, M.L., Jenkinson, N., Owen, S.L., and Aziz, T.Z. (2007). Translational principles of deep brain stimulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 623–635. Lachaux, J.P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J., and Varela, F.J. (1999). Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 194–208. Lisanby, S.H., and Belmaker, R.H. (2000). Animal models of the mechanisms of action of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (RTMS): comparisons with electroconvulsive shock (ECS). Depress. Anxiety 12, 178–187. Niessing, J., Ebisch, B., Schmidt, K.E., Niessing, M., Singer, W., and Galuske, R.A. (2005). Hemodynamic signals correlate tightly with synchronized gamma oscillations. Science 309, 948–951. Oliveri, M., and Calvo, G. (2003). Increased visual cortical excitability in ecstasy users: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 1136–1138. Oliviero, A., Strens, L.H., Di Lazzaro, V., Tonali, P.A., and Brown, P. (2003). Persistent effects of high frequency repetitive TMS on the coupling between motor areas in the human. Exp. Brain Res. 149, 107–113. Pascual-Leone, A., Tormos, J.M., Keenan, J., Tarazona, F., Canete, C., and Catala, M.D. (1998). Study and modulation of human cortical excitability with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 15, 333–343. Pascual-Leone, A., Walsh, V., and Rothwell, J. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience—virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10, 232–237. Patton, H.D., and Amassian, V.E. (1954). Single- and multiple-unit analysis of cortical stage of pyramidal tract activation. J. Neurophysiol. 17, 345–363. Paus, T. (2005). Inferring causality in brain images: a perturbation approach. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1109–1114. Pereda, E., Quiroga, R.Q., and Bhattacharya, J. (2005). Nonlinear multivariate analysis of neurophysiological signals. Prog. Neurobiol. 77, 1–37. Pesaran, B., Pezaris, J.S., Sahani, M., Mitra, P.P., and Andersen, R.A. (2002). Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 805–811. Reich, D.S., Mechler, F., Purpura, K.P., and Victor, J.D. (2000). Interspike intervals, receptive fields, and information encoding in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, 1964–1974. Ridding, M.C., and Rothwell, J.C. (2007). Is there a future for therapeutic use of transcranial magnetic stimulation? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 559–567. Liu, J., and Newsome, W.T. (2006). Local field potential in cortical area MT: stimulus tuning and behavioral correlations. J. Neurosci. 26, 7779–7790. Romei, V., Brodbeck, V., Michel, C., Amedi, A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Thut, G. (2008). Spontaneous fluctuations in posterior alpha-band EEG activity reflect variability in excitability of human visual areas. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2010–2018. Logothetis, N.K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453, 869–878. Ruff, C.C., Blankenburg, F., Bjoertomt, O., Bestmann, S., Freeman, E., Haynes, J.D., Rees, G., Josephs, O., Deichmann, R., and Driver, J. (2006). 302 Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. Neuron State-Dependent Neural Effects of TMS Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on human retinotopic visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 16, 1479–1488. Stevens, C.F., and Zador, A.M. (1998). Input synchrony and the irregular firing of cortical neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 210–217. Saalmann, Y.B., Pigarev, I.N., and Vidyasagar, T.R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science 316, 1612–1615. Stewart, L.M., Walsh, V., and Rothwell, J.C. (2001). Motor and phosphene thresholds: a transcranial magnetic stimulation correlation study. Neuropsychologia 39, 415–419. Sack, A.T., Kohler, A., Bestmann, S., Linden, D.E., Dechent, P., Goebel, R., and Baudewig, J. (2007). Imaging the brain activity changes underlying impaired visuospatial judgments: simultaneous fMRI, TMS, and behavioral studies. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2841–2852. Strens, L.H., Oliviero, A., Bloem, B.R., Gerschlager, W., Rothwell, J.C., and Brown, P. (2002). The effects of subthreshold 1 Hz repetitive TMS on cortico-cortical and interhemispheric coherence. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1279–1285. Salinas, F.S., Lancaster, J.L., and Fox, P.T. (2007). Detailed 3D models of the induced electric field of transcranial magnetic stimulation coils. Phys. Med. Biol. 52, 2879–2892. Tehovnik, E.J., Tolias, A.S., Sultan, F., Slocum, W.M., and Logothetis, N.K. (2006). Direct and indirect activation of cortical neurons by electrical microstimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 512–521. Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Gerloff, C., and Hummel, F.C. (2009). Spontaneous locally restricted EEG alpha activity determines cortical excitability in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologia 47, 284–288. Terao, Y., and Ugawa, Y. (2002). Basic mechanisms of TMS. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 19, 322–343. Theodore, W.H. (2003). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in epilepsy. Epilepsy Curr. 3, 191–197. Siegel, M., and Konig, P. (2003). A functional gamma-band defined by stimulus-dependent synchronization in area 18 of awake behaving cats. J. Neurosci. 23, 4251–4260. Thomson, D.J. (1982). Spectrum estimation and harmonic analysis. Proc. IEEE 70, 1055–1096. Silvanto, J., and Muggleton, N.G. (2008). New light through old windows: moving beyond the ‘‘virtual lesion’’ approach to transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 39, 549–552. Van Der Werf, Y.D., Sadikot, A.F., Strafella, A.P., and Paus, T. (2006). The neural response to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex. II. Thalamocortical contributions. Exp. Brain Res. 175, 246–255. Silvanto, J., Muggleton, N.G., Cowey, A., and Walsh, V. (2007). Neural adaptation reveals state-dependent effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 1874–1881. Wespatat, V., Tennigkeit, F., and Singer, W. (2004). Phase sensitivity of synaptic modifications in oscillating cells of rat visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 9067–9075. Skottun, B.C., De Valois, R.L., Grosof, D.H., Movshon, J.A., Albrecht, D.G., and Bonds, A.B. (1991). Classifying simple and complex cells on the basis of response modulation. Vision Res. 31, 1079–1086. Ziemann, U., Tam, A., Butefisch, C., and Cohen, L.G. (2002). Dual modulating effects of amphetamine on neuronal excitability and stimulation-induced plasticity in human motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1308–1315. Neuron 62, 291–303, April 30, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 303
* Your assessment is very important for improving the work of artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
advertisement