Adolescent Shyness and Social Relationships Avh Nejra BesicL_K210409.indd 1

Adolescent Shyness and Social Relationships Avh Nejra BesicL_K210409.indd   1
Adolescent Shyness and Social Relationships
This dissertation is dedicated to my
mother Zifa and my father Aziz.
“Scientists have found the gene for shyness.
They would have found it years ago, but it was
hiding behind a couple of other genes”.
JONATHAN KATZ
Örebro Studies in Psychology 16
Nejra BeŠic´
At First Blush:
The Impact of Shyness on Early
Adolescents’ Social Worlds
© Nejra Bešic,
´ 2009
Title: At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness
on Early Adolescent’s Social Worlds
Publisher: Örebro University 2009
www.publications.oru.se
Editor: Heinz Merten
[email protected]
Printer: intellecta infolog, V Frölunda 04/2009
issn 1651-1328
isbn 978-91-7668-667-6
A bst r act
Shyness as a behavioral characteristic has been in focus of research in psychology for
a number of decades. Adolescent shyness has, however, been relatively overlooked
compared with studies conducted on children and adults. This dissertation
concentrated on adolescent shyness, aiming to attain a better comprehension about
how shyness during this developmental phase might affect, and be affected by social
relationships. The first aim of this dissertation was to study in which way shyness
influences and is influenced by significant people in adolescents’ lives: peers, friends,
and parents. Study III showed that shy youths socialized each other over time into
becoming even more shy. Study VI demonstrated that youths’ shyness affected
parenting behaviors, more so than parent’s behaviors affected youth shyness. The
second aim of this dissertation was to investigate what shyness means for
adolescents’ choices of relationships with friends, whereas the third aim focused on
whether adolescents’ ways of dealing with peers would have consequences for their
internal and external adjustment. As Study I showed, youths might take on offputting, startling appearances in order to cope with their shyness. This strategy
seemed, nonetheless, not particularly successful for the shy youths in terms of
emotional adjustment. Study III showed that adolescents who were shy tended to
choose others similar to themselves in shyness as friends. Study II showed that
shyness might indeed have some positive implications for adolescent development, as
it was found to serve a protective role in the link between advanced maturity and
various types of problem behaviors. Overall, the findings point to some gender
differences regarding all of the abovementioned processes. In sum then, the studies in
this dissertation show that even though youths’ shy, socially fearful characteristics
affect their emotional adjustment and those around them, shy youths are part of a
larger social arena where they are active agents in shaping their own development.
Although adolescent shyness might be linked with several negative outcomes,
however, it might be other people’s reactions to socially fearful behaviors that help
create and/or maintain these outcomes over time.
Keywords: shyness, adolescence, social relationships, friends, peers, parents, social
identity, socialization, problem behaviors
[email protected]
A cknowl edgement s
Saying that this dissertation would not have happened without my supervisor
Margaret Kerr is by all means an understatement. Ever since she supervised my
bachelors’ thesis and patiently listened to my ramblings and ideas about research,
Margaret has been my guiding light throughout the entire process of everything from
generating ideas, writing the studies, providing insight about writing principles, to
advising me in all matters regarding research (and life!). Margaret, you have showed
me a great deal of forbearance, tolerance, and kindness during this entire time. You
are also the one who introduced me to the mere idea of doing research, and as to this
day I am grateful for your faith in me both at that time and ever since then. I also
want to thank my co-supervisor Håkan Stattin for his general questions, comments,
and ideas, which have been greatly appreciated. Håkan, you have been the comic
relief throughout these four years, and your presence at the working place has
contributed greatly to my positive feelings for research in general, and psychology in
particular. And a huge thanks to both Margaret and Håkan for letting me run away
from time to time to play some music. I hugely appreciate it!
A second round of applause goes to my wonderful colleagues at the Center
for Developmental Research. You guys have all been an essential reason to get up in
the mornings during the dark Swedish winters and crawl my way to work. Our
lunchtime discussions, all of which I would never mention anything about in more
sophisticated circles, have often been the highlights of my day (unless I had some
fabulous research ideas, that is!). A special thanks to Vilmante Pakalniskiene, who
has been my source of all things pleasant during my years at CDR. Lauree TiltonWeaver: you are a complete sweetheart (with a touch of rock’n’roll!), and have
showed me great kindness during moments of need; I am truly thankful for that. I
also want to express my appreciation to the rest of the people at CDR, both former
and current colleagues: without all of your insightful comments, questions, remarks,
and all other kinds of assistance, this dissertation would most probably have been
even worse. Thank you Stefan Persson, Andreas Persson, Nikolaus Koutakis,
Therese Skoog, Ylva Svensson, Terése Glatz, Monika Geisor, Mats Larsson, William
Burk, Viveca Olofsson, Maria Tillfors, Göran Jutengren, Selma Salihovi, Fumiko
Kakihara, and everyone else I might have forgotten to mention.
My mom Zifa, dad Aziz, grandma Vasvija, and my brother Edvin: thank you
for your never-ending support throughout my doctoral student years (and always!).
You are the best family one could ever ask for, and I love you all dearly. Mama i tata:
rijei nemogu da izraze koliko vas volim! Bez vas, nebi bila ono što jesam. Uvijek ste
me slušali i podravali, i podsticali da radim ono što mi duša eli. Hvala vam za sve,
ovaj doktorski rad posveujem vama.
My friends Richard Johansson, Malin Sundin, Karin S. Nordberg, Tobias &
Linda Axelsson, Mikael Karlsson: you have seen me go from my social self, to a
complete nerd and a recluse. And yet you have remained my friends during this
madness. Thank you for being wonderful.
My particular recognition goes to my dearest, sweetest Maarten, to whom I
owe so much. You are the most amazing partner, colleague, and friend one could
ever ask for. You have assisted me with ideas, helped me with statistics, and listened
to me when I have needed it the most. I thank you so much for being there for me
during this stressful time. I hope we find other topics of conversations now that our
dissertations are done.
Finally, I want to acknowledge all the people involved in the research projects I
have been privileged to partake in. All of you who collected the data, generated ideas
for questionnaires and measures, and helped out with the big and the small things;
and to all students, parents, and teachers who were willing to participate: without all
of you, this dissertation would not have been possible.
L i st of st udi es
This dissertation is based on the following studies, which will hereafter be referred to
in the text by their Roman numerals.
Study I
Beši, N., & Kerr, M. (2009). Punks, Goths, and other eye-catching
peer crowds: Do they fulfill a function for shy youths? Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 19, 113-121.
Study II
Beši, N., Kerr, M., & Tilton-Weaver, L. Shyness as protective factor
in the link between advanced maturity and early adolescent problem
behavior. Manuscript in preparation.
Study III
Beši, N., Selfhout, M. H. W., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. Shyness as basis
for friendship selection and socialization in a youth social network.
Manuscript in preparation.
Study IV
Beši, N., & Kerr, M. Shy adolescents’ perceptions of parental
overcontrol and emotional coldness: Examining bidirectional links.
Manuscript to be resubmitted for review.
Study I has been reprinted with kind permission from Journal of Research on
Adolescence.
T ABLE OF CONTENTS
I Introduction........................................................................................... 13
The defi nition of shyness.............................................................................14
What is shyness and why should we study it? .............................................14
What is NOT shyness? ................................................................................16
Related terms ..............................................................................................17
Overlap between shyness and related terms ................................................20
Variations in shyness...................................................................................22
Temperamental bases of shyness .................................................................22
Subgroups of shyness: Dual approaches ......................................................23
Gender differences ......................................................................................24
Cultural aspects and differences ................................................................. 25
Shyness and social worlds ...........................................................................26
Implications of shyness on social relationships ...........................................26
Shyness as a barrier for social interactions ..................................................27
Situations that evoke shyness ......................................................................29
Shyness and relationships in adolescence ....................................................29
Summary ....................................................................................................36
The aim of this dissertation ........................................................................38
II Method .................................................................................................39
Participants and procedure .........................................................................39
Sample 1 ..................................................................................................39
Sample 2 ..................................................................................................40
Measures.....................................................................................................41
Shyness and other characteristics ................................................................42
Maturity .....................................................................................................42
Social Relationships ....................................................................................43
Peers ........................................................................................................43
Romantic partners...................................................................................44
Friends ....................................................................................................44
Parents ....................................................................................................44
Adjustment..................................................................................................46
Emotional adjustment .............................................................................46
Problematic Behaviors.................................................................................46
III Results ................................................................................................49
Study I ........................................................................................................49
Study II .......................................................................................................50
Study III ......................................................................................................52
Study IV ......................................................................................................54
IV Discussion ..............................................................................................57
Findings and previous research ...................................................................57
Strengths and limitations ............................................................................60
What IS shyness? ........................................................................................62
Positive aspects of shyness...........................................................................63
The developmental significance of shyness in adolescence ..........................64
Future directions .........................................................................................64
What should be done about shyness? ..........................................................66
Closing remarks ..........................................................................................68
V
References ............................................................................................71
I I nt r oduct i on
“I’m shy and can’t for the life of me barge around and slap people on the back. I sit
in a corner by myself and am tickled to death when someone comes over to talk to
me”.
Alan Ladd, actor
My interest in shyness came from working as a music teacher. I was completing my
last year of internship at a local high school, teaching young people to play in
ensembles and sing in a choir. In Sweden, high school students can select an aesthetic
track, which means that besides the regular set of subjects, they also get their choice
of music, dance, or drama classes. It was a wonderful job teaching young people to
play and sing, but I noticed early on that a big part of the work was in fact about
educating them to relax in front of other people and be able to express themselves. It
was as if the primary goal of my lessons was about helping these youths learn social
skills and how to function in a group, as opposed to singing or playing together. It
struck me as odd that people would come to my classes, looking like they spent three
hours in front of a mirror trying to appear as strange as possible and drawing a lot
of attention to themselves, and yet they seemed to be very shy. How could that be, I
wondered, being a self-proclaimed non-shy person? As human beings, we are
undeniably biologically programmed to sense fear in certain conditions. The function
of fear is to caution us about some impending threat or danger, and to prepare us to
avoid it. It seemed to me that fear was a driving force in how my shy pupils would
approach any task I would give them, that would involve some kind of exposure in
front of others, or potential judgment or evaluation on my behalf. In fact, when I did
start thinking about it, it seemed to me that in my own past during high school or
my music academy years, many people that I knew that played music, wrote it, and
performed it in some way – seemed to be reserved, timid people. If one were shy, it
occurred to me, wouldn’t it be easier to pick a track in life where one does not have
to expose oneself to such an extent if one truly finds it all too discomforting? And
yet, when googling the matter of shy performers, I realized that the entertainment
business is full of self-confessed shy people, even those who really stick out with their
appearance and their views. Shyness, it seems then, could also be something inside
the individual, something that is not necessarily connected to how people are
perceived by others. Perhaps shyness is not only in the eye of the beholder, but also
largely within the persons themselves. So if we perceive people as shy, and they view
themselves that way, how does shyness affect people’s social lives? That is, in which
ways does it matter regarding the surrounding people and the society in general that
one is shy? These, among many other questions, were what spurred my interest in
the subject of shyness.
In this dissertation, I concentrate on early adolescents. Knowledge about
shyness in this period of development, however, is far clearer and more thoroughly
researched for children and adults. Thus, I include these populations in the literature
review as well, as the collected knowledge about shyness in all phases of life should
not be neglected. Generally, I focus on the role of shyness in social relationships of
early adolescents. Some of the primary issues in the dissertation concern how shyness
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 13
affects, and is in turn affected by people who are key players in the social arena of
young people’s lives, jointly referred to as adolescents’ social worlds: peers, friends,
and parents. I explore how others in adolescents’ social worlds react to shyness, and
whether those reactions seem, in turn, to influence shyness. In addition, I investigate
the implications of shyness on adolescent adjustment. Finally, I explore the role of
gender regarding all of these issues.
T he def i ni t i on of shyness
What is shyness and why should we study it?
“People assume you can’t be shy and be on television. They’re wrong.”
Diane Sawyer, television reporter
As one starts to think about shyness in a more systematic fashion, it becomes rather
apparent that it will not be as simple as one might have expected in the first place. In
fact, the discussion about the meaning of shyness, as I have come to understand it,
must be separated into a semantic and an empirical one. The everyday, lay meaning
of the word shyness is diverse. According to Merriam-Webster Online, the word
shyness stems from a 12th century Old English word, which meant “to frighten off”
(Merriam-Webster online dictionary). Today, being shy means: “easily frightened or
timid; disposed to avoid a person or thing (publicity shy); hesitant in committing
oneself (circumspect); sensitively diffident or retiring (reserved); expressive of such a
state or nature (e.g., a shy smile); secluded, hidden” (Merriam-Webster online
dictionary). Seeing as it is a commonly used word, most people have an idea about
what shy means and how a shy person is, or in which way they would behave. To be
sure, when asking my friends how they view a shy person, many of them think of
someone who is seemingly timid, spends most of their time on their own, and does
not have many friends. Others think of a socially awkward person, someone who
gets embarrassed easily and blushes all over when being with other people. Certainly
all of them get some associations. Thus, shyness as a lay term can mean everything
from being timid, or shying away from meeting new people, to showing physical
signs of shyness such as blushing. The complexity of the everyday term is partly due
to the fact that most of us can admit to being shy at one point or another in our
lives. When asking people if they were shy sometime during their lifetime, Zimbardo
and colleagues learned that more than eighty percent answered yes (Zimbardo,
1977). In this way, shyness can be something situation-bound that most people
experience. A common situation that might evoke feelings of shyness in a lot of
people is for example holding a speech in front of one’s class. Shyness, however, can
also mean different things to different people (Zimbardo, 1977). Consequently, the
semantic meaning of the word shyness is diverse and can interfere with the
understanding of the empirical definition of the term. In psychology, the concept of
shyness has received a lot of attention from the late 1960’s and forward, and what is
meant by the lay term does not necessarily correspond to the empirical view of
shyness – which can potentially be challenging for our understanding of it.
14 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
The terms in the psychology literature defining shyness are just as many as are
the debates surrounding the use of them. Researchers have admittedly concluded
that “shyness is a fuzzy concept” (Zimbardo, 1977, p. 13), and “although there are
many theories, nobody knows exactly what shyness is” (Carducci, 1999, p. 5). Thus,
it seems that “shyness is not a precise term” (Crozier, 2000, p. 2). As mentioned
previously, most people can experience shyness when coming across social situations
they might find challenging for some reason (Russell, Cutrona, & Jones, 1986;
Zimbardo, 1977). We might feel shy when being in novel social situations, or when
approaching a stranger. Or we might win an essay competition in high school and
feel very shy when having to read our work out loud in front of the entire class. This
type of shyness, so-called situational shyness or state shyness, is different from
shyness as an enduring behavioral characteristic (Asendorpf, 1990c). Simply put,
state shyness refers to intraindividual differences in shyness (Asendorpf, 1990c). For
a person with shy characteristics, however, most new people, places, and situations
seem to evoke an inward feeling of shyness. In order to be considered dispositionally
shy, one should experience problems connected with shyness on a frequent basis,
more intensely, and in a wider variety of social settings compared with people who
do not label themselves shy (Cheek & Watson, 1989). Thus, according to some
scholars, to be considered dispositional, shyness should be experienced as a problem
for the individual. This type of shyness is often referred to as trait shyness, and
focuses on interindividual differences (Asendorpf, 1990c). Thus, there seems to be
some confusion regarding how shyness is empirically defined.
In an article attempting to create debate about the problematic features of the
word shyness, it was claimed that as psychologists, we need to invent a different term
for shyness (Harris, 1984). There is no such thing as real shyness, claimed the
author, but one term that is being used in the everyday language, and another which
is employed by psychologists (Harris, 1984). Imposing our psychological definition
of shyness on the everyday word is an example of psychological imperialism. In an
attempt to meet this critique, a study was conducted where one hundred eighty
female participants (ages 14-58) were asked about their perceptions of the meaning
of being shy (Cheek & Watson, 1989). The authors concluded that the participants
provided information very close to that of the psychological definition of shyness, in
that shyness was defined as having three components that arise in social situations: a
somatic, a behavioral, and a cognitive component (Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999;
Cheek & Watson, 1989). The somatic component involves having physiological and
affective-emotional symptoms such as blushing, trembling, feeling upset, and so forth
(Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999; Cheek & Watson, 1989). The behavioral
component includes quietness, awkward conversations, nonverbal behavior such as
gaze aversion, withdrawing from social contacts, and avoiding social interactions
(Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999; Cheek & Watson, 1989). Finally, the cognitive
component involves thoughts and worries, such as fearing rejection or being selfconscious (Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999; Cheek & Watson, 1989). Some cooccurrence of the indicators specified by this three-component model has been
identified, in that forty-three percent acknowledged only having symptoms
corresponding to one component, thirty-seven percent recognized having symptoms
from two components, and twelve percent stated having symptoms relating to all of
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 15
the three components (Cheek & Watson, 1989). It was concluded that individuals
are shy if they have problems regarding at least one of the components (Cheek &
Watson, 1989). In this way, the authors claim, even individuals who do not
experience all of the aforementioned symptoms as problematic but still claim to be
shy are validated in their own view of their shyness (Cheek & Watson, 1989). Similar
findings confirm the notion that lay persons’ judgments of shyness, both self- and
other-reports, refer to corresponding behavioral criteria to those of the psychological
definition of shyness (Asendorpf, 1992). The lay term of shyness is hence compatible
to its definition in developmental research.
This characterization of shyness also corresponds to other definitions, such as
wariness in new social encounters and novel places, and with unfamiliar people
(Asendorpf, 1991; Cheek & Buss, 1981; Cheek & Watson, 1989). It also relates to
shyness being “a tendency to avoid social interactions and to fail to participate
appropriately in social situations” (Pilkonis, 1977b, p. 596), or the tendency to feel
tense, worried, or awkward during social interactions, especially with unknown
individuals (Cheek, Melchior, & Carpentieri, 1986). This definition also parallels
shyness being identified as “a tendency to respond with heightened anxiety, selfconsciousness, and reticence in a variety of social contexts; a person high in the trait
of shyness will experience greater arousal than a person low in shyness independent
of the level of interpersonal threat in the situation” (Jones, Briggs, & Smith, 1986, p.
630). Consequently, a number of different explanations or definitions of shyness
show conceptual equivalence when intuitively compared with each other. The main
feature of shyness, nonetheless, seems to be a fear of novel social situations; a feature
that all of the abovementioned definitions encompass.
Besides shyness representing wariness in social situations, there are some
additional features of shyness that need to be taken into account. Namely, being shy
also involves self-conscious behaviors in situations where one might be socially
evaluated by others (Pilkonis, 1977b). As such, shyness is highly related to the desire
for social approval by other people, and a fear of negative evaluation and rejection
(Jackson, Towson, & Narduzzi, 1997; Jones et al., 1986; Leary & Kowalski, 1993;
Miller, 1995; Pilkonis, 1977a; Watson & Friend, 1969). According to some
scholars, this fear of social evaluation is essential to dispositional shyness
(Asendorpf, 1987). Indeed, research shows that shyness can even be elicited by the
mere anticipation of social evaluation (Asendorpf, 1989). In sum then, shyness is not
just a way of being and thinking: shyness can impact individual behaviors as well
(Crozier, 2001).
What is NOT shyness?
There are some characteristics or behaviors that might be part of people’s intuitive
notions of shyness, but they are not usually considered part of the construct. For
example, shyness should not be equated with a lack of sociability, which in turn is
defined as a preference for being with others rather than alone (Bruch, Giordano, &
Pearl, 1986; Buss, 1986; Cheek & Buss, 1981; Schmidt & Fox, 1994; Schmidt &
Robinson, 1992). Similarly, shyness should not be mistaken for introversion either,
as introverted individuals are not necessarily shy (Carducci, 1999). Being shy is not
16 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
merely being unsociable, as shyness and sociability have been found to vary on trait
level (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Being shy versus being unsociable are two distinct
personality dispositions that differ from one another in terms of correlations with
self-esteem and self-consciousness (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Shy individuals report
worse self-esteem and higher self-consciousness than those who view themselves as
sociable (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Thus, the standoffishness and the bashfulness often
regarded as stereotypical shy behaviors might instead be consequences of shyness.
In addition, shyness is not identical to embarrassment. People often use the
word embarrassed when they describe a shy person in everyday language, and being
shy can mean being easily embarrassed in layman’s terms (Crozier, 1990).
Embarrassment refers to feeling uncomfortable, accompanied by a loss of self-esteem
and physical reactions such as blushing, mental confusion, and so forth (Crozier,
1990). It can also refer to emotional arousal suffering from a sense of exposure,
followed by a feeling of insufficiency or abashment (Miller, 1986). Scholars have
argued that shyness and embarrassment blend into each other when people are faced
with undeniable prospects of a predicament that is yet to happen (Miller, 1986).
Shyness and embarrassment have, however, been found to differ on trait level, and in
other important aspects (Miller, 1995). For example, embarrassable people get easily
concerned with the suitability of their behaviors according to the general norms, and
are more motivated to avoid rejection by other people (Miller, 1995). Shyness,
however, is predicted by low social skills and self-confidence (Miller, 1995). In that
sense, shyness is more linked to being competent in social interactions, whereas
embarrassability is linked with appropriateness in such situations (Miller, 1995). The
link between shyness and embarrassment is, nonetheless, not fully clear.
Finally, shyness may be related to shame (Crozier, 1999), but should not be
equated with it. Shame can also be thought of as a multifaceted pattern of thoughts,
behaviors, and bodily reactions similar to that of shyness. Feelings of shame,
however, include thinking about the self in somewhat different ways than typical
“shy” feelings. Namely, the self is both the subject and the object of evaluation when
feeling ashamed (Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow, 1996). That is, people can feel
ashamed whether they are with others or alone, whereas shyness typically arises in
social situations (Tangney et al., 1996). Nonetheless, there are some similarities
between shyness and shame. For example, blushing is often a physical reaction to
shame, as is for shyness (Pilkonis, 1977b). The association between shame and
shyness is, even so, not firmly established (Crozier, 1999). In sum then, even though
certain behaviors such as embarrassment, shame, or lack of sociability might be
regarded as typical shy behaviors in everyday terms, they are considered different
than shyness in the psychological literature.
Related terms
There are several additional terms that have either been coined to correspond to the
lay term of shyness, are highly correlated with shyness, or sometimes used as if they
mean shyness. These concepts often show considerable overlap with the definition of
shyness (Crozier, 2000). Even though the aim of this dissertation is not to discuss all
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 17
possible related terms to that of shyness, I intend to give a short description of those
I feel are relevant or I have referred to in my studies.
Behavioral inhibition. A concept very close to that of shyness is behavioral inhibition
to the unfamiliar. Children classified as inhibited are often distinguishable from other
children in that they act distressed, avoidant, and with subdued affect (Kagan, 1999;
Kagan, Reznick, & Snidman, 1988). This conception incorporates the idea of social
wariness toward novelties, such as people, situations, and events. As such, it can be
distinguished in very small children (Kagan, 1999). For example, when faced with
new places, events, or people, infants rated as behaviorally inhibited are discernible
from non-inhibited counterparts regarding a number of biological aspects, such as
crying, heightened heart rate, withdrawal, timidity, and inhibition of vocalization and
motor behaviors (Garcia-Coll, Kagan, & Reznick, 1984; Kagan, Reznick, &
Snidman, 1988). As being inhibited implies showing avoidant behaviors in one or
more contexts, only a proportion of shy children might be classified as inhibited, and
some children categorized as inhibited might not be shy with strangers (Kagan,
1999). Being focused on behaviors in new social situations, however, makes
inhibition a somewhat different notion than that of shyness, as for example no
conception of self-awareness in social situations is included (Gest, 1997). Inhibition
in the early years of life is moderately stable over time (Garcia-Coll et al., 1984; Gest,
1997; Kagan, Reznick, & Snidman, 1988; Moehler et al., 2008). Early behavioral
inhibition is related to anxiety disorders later on in life (Schwartz, Snidman, &
Kagan, 1999; Van Ameringen, Mancini, & Oakman, 1998). Inhibited children differ
from non-inhibited children on other interesting features, such as family history of
hay fever (Kagan, Snidman, Julia-Sellers, & Johnson, 1991), mothers with pregnancy
during times with reduced daylight (Gortmaker, Kagan, Caspi, & Silva, 1997), and a
tendency to have blue eyes (Rosenberg & Kagan, 1987), although the latter might
only be true for boys (Coplan, Coleman, & Rubin, 1998). Thus, there are apparent
biological differences between inhibited and non-inhibited children, and scholars
have argued that behavioral inhibition is a basic temperament (Kagan & Snidman,
1991). The view of behavioral inhibition is often a categorical one, however; a person
is either inhibited or non-inhibited (Crozier, 2000). Thus, behavioral inhibition, as it
has been defined, is highly similar to the basic idea of shyness.
Social anxiety. Another term often associated with shyness is social anxiety. Social
anxiety has been defined as the ongoing occurrence of uneasiness, negative ideation,
and inept performance in the expectation and conduct of interpersonal transaction
(Hartman, 1986). Social anxiety, then, occurs in social interactions (Blöte, Kint,
Miers, & Westenberg, 2009), and is not to be equated with for example speech
anxiety (Hartman, 1986). Besides for fear of negative evaluation, social anxiety also
involves avoidance of social situations, and perceived social distress in interactions
(La Greca & Lopez, 1998). Socially anxious individuals should be impaired to some
degree in three routes of experience: feelings, behaviors, and cognitions (Hartman,
1986). In this sense, social anxiety almost completely corresponds to the definition of
shyness as having three components that need be present for a classification (Cheek
& Watson, 1989), with the main difference that socially anxious individuals need to
18 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
be impaired in each of the three components simultaneously. High social anxiety has
been found to occur in around eighteen percent of the population (Dell’Osso et al.,
2003). Social anxiety can also be viewed as an uneasiness that arises before a social
situation (Carducci, 1999). Accordingly, even though the concept of shyness does
incorporate social anxiety, it also includes behaviors, thoughts, and emotions during
and after social interactions (Carducci, 1999). It is understandable, then, that some
scholars would see social anxiety as being a small part of the broader definition of
shyness (Leary & Buckley, 2000). Social anxiety, it seems then, is highly related to
shyness.
Social withdrawal. Another correlate of shyness is social withdrawal. Some scholars
see social withdrawal as a developmental outcome of behavioral inhibition (Fox,
Henderson, Marshall, Nichols, & Ghera, 2005). This concept is mostly studied in
childhood. Social withdrawal has been defined as a preference for spending time
alone as opposed to being with others (Coplan, Prakash, O’Neil, & Armer, 2004).
Withdrawal can, however, also signal rejection, exclusion, or isolation from the peer
group, and thus depend solely on a child’s relation to its social world (Boivin, Hymel,
& Bukowski, 1995; Gazelle & Ladd, 2003). Social withdrawal should, nevertheless,
not be misinterpreted as social disinterest (Asendorpf, 1990b), and should be
differentiated from active isolation, which indicates a process of children being alone
around others because they are rejected by their peers (Rubin & Asendorpf, 1993).
Instead, socially withdrawn children are those who isolate themselves from the
group, due to factors such as anxiety, lack of social skills, and so forth (Rubin &
Asendorpf, 1993). Taken together, then, social withdrawal could be a consequence
of shyness.
Social reticence. Social reticence is a correlate of shyness, but the term is often used in
literature where children who are observed in play with other children have been
found to remain unoccupied in such situations, and hover around others (Coplan,
Rubin, Fox, Calkins, & Stewart, 1994). According to some scholars, however,
reticence is an end result of shyness (Carducci, 1999). Others, in addition, have used
this term interchangeably with that of behavioral inhibition (Rubin, Cheah, & Fox,
2001). Reticence can be described by several socially unsuccessful behaviors, such as
refraining from social participation, creating self-fulfilling prophecies about social
failure, engaging in social contacts with programmatic activity (such as e.g., learning
to nod, and act in a certain way), showing signs of nervous mannerisms (such as e.g.,
hesitant speech), and a high sensitivity to criticism (Phillips, 1997). As such, the idea
and the consequences of social reticence highly resemble those of shyness.
Social Anxiety Disorder. Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD), more commonly known as
social phobia, is a clinical internalizing disorder defined as “a marked and persistent
fear of one or more social situations in which the person is exposed to unfamiliar
people or to possible scrutiny by others” (p. 416) in the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders – Fourth Edition (APA, 1994). Social phobia is
debilitating for the individual, and it tends to precede other disorders such as
substance abuse and depression (Rapee & Spence, 2004). Some debate exists in the
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 19
literature about whether social anxiety disorder is to be considered as a continuous
construct, and whether it, similar to shyness, might comprise several subtypes (Rapee
& Spence, 2004). Common apprehensions of social phobics include speaking and
eating in front of others, using public bathrooms, and engaging in social interactions
(Beidel & Turner, 1999). Scholars have claimed a high similarity of this definition to
that of shyness (Heiser, Turner, & Beidel, 2003; Turner, Beidel, & Townsley, 1990).
Social phobia is, however, much less common than shyness, with prevalence rates
ranging from less than half a percent in Taiwan, to around seventeen percent on the
island of Gotland, Sweden (Furmark et al., 1999). Thus, even though there are many
similarities between social phobia and shyness, social phobia is far more problematic
in nature.
Overlap between shyness and related terms
These related terms often show substantial overlap with the definition of shyness.
The abundance of different definitions is, however, not uncomplicated (Crozier,
2000). For example, some researchers regard shyness as a form of social withdrawal,
in that shyness is motivated by concerns of social evaluation in new situations
(Rubin & Asendorpf, 1993). Social withdrawal as a term does not capture shyness
entirely, however, as shy people have been found to adopt sociable and extraverted
strategies in order to cope with their shyness (Rubin & Asendorpf, 1993). In
addition, behavioral inhibition might also be viewed as a type of withdrawal,
characterized by being alone and withdrawing from new social situations and places
(Rubin & Asendorpf, 1993). For others, behavioral inhibition, shyness, and
withdrawal are principally analogous concepts (Beidel & Turner, 1999). Even
though inhibition can be viewed as a concept different than shyness, however,
researchers have argued that it is essentially similar to shyness in important aspect
(Crozier, 2000). For example, children studied in inhibition studies get upset when
meeting new people, they are hesitant in approaching adults, and they show a
tendency to hover around other children without joining in play – characteristics
which are found for shy individuals as well (Crozier, 2000). Others even view
behavioral inhibition as one of the core features of shyness (Leary & Buckley, 2000).
Thus, according to some scholars, both shyness and behavioral inhibition can be
distinguished as various kinds of social withdrawal.
Others have hypothesized about the link between shyness and social anxiety.
For example, it has been suggested that the concept of shyness should be limited to a
specific syndrome, which includes experiencing anxiety and concurrently showing
hesitation and awkwardness, or inhibition (Leary, 1986; Leary & Buckley, 2000).
According to this view then, social anxiety is not the same thing as shyness but a
broader notion (Crozier, 2000). Others assert, however, that the cognitive
component in itself is central to understanding what shyness is, as behavioral
problems linked with shyness present a minor problem for some people’s perception
of their own social fears (Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999). A contrasting view is that
of social anxiety being a part of shyness, which in turn is considered as a broader
concept (Leary & Buckley, 2000). Thus, there are divergent views regarding the link
between social anxiety and shyness, and this issue needs further investigation.
20 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Lastly, there have been several different hypotheses regarding the link between
shyness and social anxiety disorder, or social phobia. Some have argued that lack of
social fears and social phobia are essentially on the same continuum but on the
opposite ends, where social phobia indicates the strongest type of shyness (Turner et
al., 1990). A similar hypothesis is that shyness is a mild form of social phobia
(Marshall & Lipsett, 1994), or that shyness could be viewed as overlapping or
related to social phobia (Heiser et al., 2003; Stemberger, Turner, Beidel, & Calhoun,
1995). Others have argued that as shyness is not listed in the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV, and is not an illness but a facet of
personality, it should be not be equated with social phobia at all (Carducci, 1999;
Crozier, 2000). The evidence for this claim comes from the notion that an individual
can experience shyness or face severe problems in their social life, without anyone else
around them noticing it (Crozier, 2000). Thus, there is surely a relation between
shyness and social phobia, but the nature of this relation still needs to be examined.
Which is then, the “most appropriate” or the “best” definition of behaviors
that pertain to social wariness and awkwardness in social situations? There is no
easy answer to this question. The different starting points for viewing shyness and its
overlapping terms can have an impact on our study of it, and are thus not
unproblematic. On the one hand, if shyness is defined solely by asking people how
they feel, we are ignoring the behavioral consequences of this phenomenon (Crozier,
2001). On the other hand, if we assume that shyness only occurs in social
interactions, we might be placing the importance solely on the salience of the
experience of those interactions, and ignoring the emotions behind them (Crozier,
2001). One common factor that researchers do seem to agree on, however, is that the
focal point of shyness is a fear of novel social situations. By focusing on reported
fears or wariness in new social situations and encounters, one is able to capture the
core of what being shy is mainly about.
In this dissertation, I have used the term behavioral inhibition intertwined
with that of shyness in Study I, as I have been of the opinion that the measure of
shyness used in that study corresponded well to the idea of inhibition. I have,
however, abandoned that term in the remaining studies even though I have
continued to use the same measure, because of additional issues. First, I have come to
believe that the term shyness is more semantically intuitive, as it is also used in
everyday language. In fact, instead of seeing it as a dilemma as others have (Harris,
1984), I have come to believe that there are definite advantages to using that term
above others. One important advantage is primarily being able to communicate more
easily to the surrounding public the nature of my research. Second, I have lacked
information about distinctive biological responses that might have measured
behavioral inhibition in a more correct way, typically assessed by laboratory
observations. Thus, shyness is the term used in the subsequent studies.
Trait shyness, however, is not a characteristic that is similar in all individuals.
On the contrary, shyness varies for different people depending on diverse types of
factors. There are, indeed, as many disparities in shyness as there are human beings
that define themselves as shy. In the next part of the introduction, I will focus on the
stability of shyness, some diversity in shyness characteristics, and variations on
shyness between genders and cultures.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 21
V ar i at i ons i n shyness
Temperamental bases of shyness
“The desire to annoy no one, to harm no one, can equally well be the sign of a just as
of an anxious disposition”.
Friedrich Nietzsche, philosopher
As shy children grow older, shyness can become a central part of who they are, both
in terms of their personality but also in how they view themselves, or their selfconcept (Crozier, 2000). Hence, shyness can develop into a stable characteristic.
Many view shyness as a trait (Asendorpf, 1989; Buss, 1986) or a basic temperament
(Buss & Plomin, 1984). A personality trait can be defined as descriptive or/and
explanatory concept, referring to long-lasting, characteristic, and general aspects
relating to an individual (Briggs, 1985; Briggs & Smith, 1986). Similarly,
temperament can be defined as individual differences that appear from early on in
life, which are stable over time, lead to predictable models of behaviors, and pertain
to a biological foundation (Crozier, 2001). Already in early work on personality
traits, shyness was identified as a basic trait. Seminal works by Mosier, Comrey,
Cattell, and Eysenck have recognized a shyness factor in the research on human
personality (Cattell, 1973; Comrey, 1965; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1969; Mosier, 1937).
Some researchers take the standpoint that shyness is a primary, unitary trait, which
cannot be divided into additional traits (Briggs, 1988). Others claim that the shyness
trait might indeed be divided into several subtypes (Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999).
Evidence supports the notion of shyness stability. Previous studies have shown, for
example, that mother-rated shyness at ages 8-10 predicts shyness in adulthood for
both men and women in an American sample (Caspi, Elder, & Bem, 1988). Similar
findings were obtained for women, but not men, in a Swedish sample of shy
individuals (Kerr, Lambert, & Bem, 1996). Other findings show that late-developing
shyness is more stable throughout adulthood than is early-developing shyness (Kerr,
2000). Research on inhibited temperaments shows that inhibited children differ from
non-inhibited counterparts on several biological facets (Kagan & Reznick, 1986;
Kagan, Reznick, & Gibbons, 1989), even though far from all children who are
classified as having inhibited temperaments remain inhibited as adults (Kagan,
2000). There are, however, not many other behaviors that exhibit long-term
persistence from childhood and throughout adulthood either (Caspi & Silva, 1995).
Finally, shyness shows a certain degree of heritability (Plomin & Daniels, 1986). The
estimates of the prevalence of shyness for adults range between twenty to forty-eight
percent (Carducci & Zimbardo, 1995; Lazarus, 1982; Zimbardo, 1977). The
number is similar for children, with thirty-eight percent considering themselves to be
shy (Lazarus, 1982). A more extreme type of shyness has been reported by fifteen
percent of the population (Schmidt & Fox, 1999). In general then, support has been
shown regarding the stability of shyness across the lifespan. As others have pointed
out, however, because humans are such complex living organisms, shyness cannot be
22 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
studied from a single, narrow approach based on either genetics, bodily processes, or
other aspects (Cheek & Briggs, 1990). Instead, the study of shyness ought to
comprise all of these approaches.
Subgroups of shyness: Dual approaches
Throughout the history of shyness research, interindividual differences have been
recognized in several ways. Attempts have thus been made to further clarify the
concept by identifying subgroups of shy individuals. For example, some scholars
maintain that shyness can be divided into public and private shyness (Pilkonis,
1977a). Publicly shy people are more concerned about behaving awkwardly in social
situations (Pilkonis, 1977a). Privately shy people, on the other hand, focus on their
own feelings of discomfort (Pilkonis, 1977a). Some empirical work supports this
notion, as studies with children show that some might act more shy when in public
settings, but not with familiar peers (Asendorpf, 1990b). Thus, shyness can be
thought of as having two sets of individual starting points: some shy individuals
might be more affected by social fears in public, whereas others might mostly focus
on their inner sensations of uneasiness.
Furthermore, others have proposed a view of shyness as early- vs. latedeveloping shyness (Bruch et al., 1986; Buss, 1980; Buss, 1986). The earlydeveloping shyness can be seen as fearful, typically emerging during the 1st year of
life, and influenced by temperamental features of wariness and emotionality (Kagan
& Reznick, 1986). As a further clarification of early and late shyness, Buss claimed
that shyness which emerges early in childhood is temperamentally fear-based. This
type of shyness is prevalent in the first 4–5 years of life, prior to children developing
the capacity to take another person’s standpoint and begin to worry how others see
them (Buss, 1986). Empirical studies of small children and toddlers support the idea
of temperamentally fear-based shyness (Kagan & Reznick, 1986). Later-developing
shyness, on the other hand, emerges in middle childhood or early adolescence, and
can be viewed as self-conscious (Bruch et al., 1986; Buss, 1980; Buss, 1986). This
type of shyness appears once children have started to think of themselves as social
objects, is based on self-consciousness rather than fear (Buss, 1986), and might be
stimulated by changes that occur during puberty (Cheek, Carpentieri, Smith,
Rierdan, & Koff, 1986). Shyness in adolescence might be embedded in the strong
self-consciousness that occurs in middle childhood and early adolescence (Bruch,
1989). Studies show that from middle childhood and forward, shyness is linked with
poor self-esteem, low social self-confidence, and poor social skills (Cheek &
Melchior, 1990; Crozier, 1981; Crozier, 1995; Jones & Russell, 1982; Lawrence &
Bennett, 1992; Miller, 1995). In an attempt to compare early- and later-developing
shyness in one study, Kerr found that shyness that emerges in adolescence was more
important for adjustment in adulthood (Kerr, 2000). Early-developing shyness was
less problematic in almost all domains regarding relationships, psychological wellbeing, and occupational and economic circumstances (Kerr, 2000). On the other
hand, later-developing shyness was related to more depressed mood, lower selfesteem, poorer attitudes about one’s appearance, lower life satisfaction, and less
positive affect (Kerr, 2000). Contrary to this view, nonetheless, it has been argued
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 23
that shyness should not be divided in early- and late-developing shyness, as these two
types of shyness are consistently correlated when measured separately (Briggs, 1988;
Briggs & Smith, 1986). Thus, some studies support the suggestion that children who
become shy later on are worse off than children who are shy in early childhood, even
though few attempts have been made to investigate this view.
Shyness has also been divided into withdrawn shyness and dependent shyness
(Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999). Withdrawn shyness is characterized by inhibition,
reticence, and avoiding social situations (Caspi et al., 1988; Cheek & Krasnoperova,
1999). Dependent shyness, on the other hand, is characterized by conforming ideas
and neutral attitudes often adopted by shy people, described as “going along to get
along” (Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999; Leary & Kowalski, 1995; Lewinsky, 1941).
These dissimilarities pertain mainly to individual differences in how shy people
approach others, or in their interpersonal styles. According to several scholars, these
might be two different patterns of behaviors or social solutions for shy people
(Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999; Crozier, 2001). Some empirical work has focused
on these subtypes by distinguishing between shy-sociable and shy-unsociable
individuals (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Mere sociability, however, might not be enough
to differentiate between the subtypes (Asendorpf & Meier, 1993; Bruch, Rivet,
Heimberg, Hunt, & McIntosh, 1999; Page, 1990; Schmidt & Fox, 1994, 1995).
Finally, some claim temperamental differences with diverse developmental significance
between conflicted and avoidant subtypes of shyness (Asendorpf, 1990a; Schmidt &
Fox, 1999). Conflicted shy children are characterized by an approach-avoidant
conflict, in that they are highly self-conscious and generally want to socialize but are
not able to due to their characteristics (Schmidt & Fox, 1999). In contrast, avoidant
shy children are typically high in avoidant but low in approach behavior, and show
avoidant and anxious behaviors towards others (Schmidt & Fox, 1999). Even
though most of these approaches have received some empirical support, the duality
of shyness still mainly remains an issue for future explorations. In sum then, some
research suggests that shyness is not a unitary concept, and some subgroups of
shyness have been recognized in previous studies.
Gender differences
Generally speaking, studies reporting mean differences on shyness often find that
girls demonstrate more shy behaviors compared with boys. This pattern has been
identified in early and late childhood (Burgess, Wojslawowicz, Rubin, Rose-Krasnor,
& Booth-LaForce, 2006; Crozier, 1995; Kim, Brody, & Murry, 2003; Lemerise,
1997), in adolescence (Zimbardo, 1977), and in adulthood (Dell’Osso et al., 2003).
These mean differences are, albeit, not always significant. In contrast, other work on
shyness has not been able to identify differences between girls and boys regarding the
development of shyness and related concepts (Booth-LaForce & Oxford, 2008;
Coplan et al., 1998; Coplan, Gavinski-Molina, Lagacé-Séguin, & Wichmann, 2001;
Coplan et al., 1994; Rubin, 1993). More notably, however, shyness can have rather
different consequences for men and women (Kerr, 2000). For example, it might be
more acceptable to be shy for women than for men, as men might be more pressured
to change their behaviors in order to fit in (Buss & Plomin, 1984; Kerr, Lambert,
24 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Stattin, & Klackenberg-Larsson, 1994). Women might be more expected to socialize
than men, however, as not interacting with others might be more accepted for men
(Kerr, 2000). Another explanation could be that as men deal with their own early
shyness, women could be given the signal that they should not prefer isolated
activities, and in that way become self-conscious about their usual preferences and
develop the self-conscious type of shyness as they grow up (Kerr, 2000). Research
confirms these ideas, showing gender differences in how shy women and men interact
with others. For example, in opposite-sex interactions, men are usually expected to
take the lead, and this has been shown to be more difficult for shy men as they tend
to look and talk less with their female peers (Pilkonis, 1977b). Shy women nod and
smile more often in conversations, are seemingly anxious about leaving a good
impression, and have a need to be pleasing (Pilkonis, 1977b). In comparison with
shy women, however, shy men report having more negative thoughts regarding
themselves in interactions with others (Bruch, Gorsky, Collins, & Berger, 1989).
Thus, shyness is linked with some concurrent differences between men and women.
Some longitudinal work focusing on gender differences in shyness has been
reported. In one study where participants were followed over the course of 35 years,
shy men married and became fathers later than non-shy men, but this was not true
for women (Kerr et al., 1996). The shy women, on the other hand, attained a lower
level of education compared with non-shy women (Kerr et al., 1996). A similar
pattern has been established elsewhere (Caspi et al., 1988). Girls with earlydeveloping shyness are still more shy than average until the age of 16, whereas boys
are not (Kerr et al., 1994). In addition, women that develop shyness early on show
signs of poorer psychological well-being and poorer self-esteem over time, whereas
men do not (Kerr, 2000). This difference between the genders was not found for
later-developing shyness, however (Kerr, 2000). Thus, gender differences in shyness
concern both to the way shy men and women interact with others, but also the social
consequences this might have concurrently and over the life span.
Cultural aspects and differences
The characteristics of shyness can be perceived differently according to culture (Kerr,
2001). On the one hand, in some societies, shyness is viewed as social stigma, both
for shy and non-shy persons. In cultures where individuality is valued, shyness is
seen as a negative trait (Kerr, 2001; Leary & Buckley, 2000). In the US, for example,
a lively and outgoing interaction style is preferred over subdued and inhibited styles
(Leary & Buckley, 2000). In such individualistic societies, shy people are generally
viewed as less friendly and likable (Zimbardo, 1977), less affectionate, warm, happy,
and physically attractive (Jones & Carpenter, 1986; Pilkonis, 1977b). This seems,
however, only to be the case in cultures where extraverted interpersonal styles are
valued, such as North America and Western Europe (Chen, Rubin, & Li, 1995;
Leary & Buckley, 2000). On the contrary, in the more collectivistic China, for
example, calm and unassertive behavior is more highly valued (Chen et al., 1998;
Pearson, 1991; Shenkar & Ronen, 1987). Chinese children who are shy and
inhibited are more acknowledged by their peers, and more likely to be regarded as
fitting for roles of admiration and leadership (Chen et al., 1995). These children are
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 25
also more encouraged to be shy by peers and parents (Chen et al., 1998; Chen et al.,
1995). Hence, shyness might have diverse consequences depending on individuals’
surrounding culture and society.
As I have shown, the term shyness is related to and shows overlap with many
different terms, all of which measure social fears in their own way. In addition,
shyness has diverse significance for different kinds of people. As shyness seems to
affect people’s way of being, surely it has great impact on one of the most important
part of most people’s lives: their social worlds.
S hyness and soci al wor l ds
Implications of shyness on social relationships
“I was really kind of shy as a child. But I would do things for attention.”
Little Richard, musician
Human beings are sociable animals. We spend most of our time in closeness to
others, and our social interactions and relationships with the people around us are a
fundamental part of most of our lives (Leary & Buckley, 2000). This propensity to
be with others might indeed stem from the fact that humans need each other more
for plain survival than other animals do (Leary & Buckley, 2000). In order for us to
have good relationships with other people, we must appear to be the kind of person
with whom others would want to have a relationship with. This can mean everything
from friendships, forming groups, and finding romantic partners, to developing
other relationships (Leary & Buckley, 2000). It is perhaps this need to belong with
someone or some other people that has lead the human kind to seek the social
acceptance of others as much as we do, even though this might not be true for all
people and all situations (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Leary & Buckley, 2000). In this
sense, nonetheless, shyness seems to get in the way with development of interpersonal
relationships (Kerr, 2001; Leary & Buckley, 2000). Scholars suggest that shyness
might be more of an issue in today’s modern society than it might have been for
people in earlier times, because nowadays we are more subjected to a continually
changing array of relationships and social interactions (Leary & Buckley, 2000). As
our society changes and the means of communication and interactions with others
constantly grow, it is of significance to understand in which way this might impact
shy individuals.
Needless to say, some people are satisfied with not having so much contacts
and interpersonal relationships (Cheek & Buss, 1981). Some individuals are
relatively unsociable, and might feel perfectly at ease with living their lives just the way
they are. Research shows that many shy people, however, are not as content with
their social lives as non-shy people (Leary & Buckley, 2000). For example, empirical
work has recognized that shy people feel more lonely compared with non-shy
individuals (Cheek & Busch, 1981; Jones, Freemon, & Goswick, 1981; Jones &
Russell, 1982; Neto, 1992). This implies that shy people themselves perceive their
loneliness as problematic (Leary & Buckley, 2000). Notably, shy people feel lonely
26 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
concerning all types of relationships, regardless of whether it is with friends and
romantic partners, within groups, or within the family (Leary & Buckley, 2000). As
shyness seems to affect many people’s social lives in a way that is perceived as
negative by the individuals themselves, it warrants further consideration.
Shyness as a barrier for social interactions
There are several things we know about the way that shyness can impact social
relationships. Generally speaking, shyness might obstruct the possibilities for
engaging in social interactions and being socially accepted (Leary & Buckley, 2000).
First, shyness might limit people’s prospects for social interactions, which in turn are
essential for the development of social relationships (Leary & Buckley, 2000). Shy
individuals tend to avoid social interactions, in order to circumvent the possible
prospect of anxiety or embarrassing situations. Research shows that shy people
interact less with others socially, both over time and on a daily basis. In general, shy
people have fewer friends, and it takes a longer time for them to develop their
friendships compared with non-shy people (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998). Moreover,
once shy individuals dare attend social gatherings, they do not talk as much with
other people and tend to spend less time at such events (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998;
Dodge, Heimberg, Nyman, & O’Brien, 1987; Himadi, Arkowitz, Hinton, & Perl,
1980; Twentyman & McFall, 1975; Watson & Friend, 1969). In addition, shy
individuals tend to feel less supported by their friends and loved ones (Jones &
Carpenter, 1986), and are more dissatisfied with their social lives in general (Neto,
1993). They also perceive themselves as less likable by others compared to non-shy
people (Jones & Carpenter, 1986; Leary, Kowalski, & Campbell, 1988; Pozo,
Carver, Wellens, & Scheier, 1991). Regarding romantic relationships, shy people go
on fewer dates, get involved in fewer sexual encounters, and are less likely to be
involved in a romantic relationship at any point in time compared with non-shy
people (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998; Jones & Carpenter, 1986; Leary & Dobbins,
1983; Prisbell, 1991; Zimbardo, 1977). Thus, shyness seems to impact the
opportunities for shy individuals to meet other people.
Second, shy people are associated with behavioral patterns that do not aid
approval and closeness by others (Leary & Buckley, 2000). That is, when shy
individuals interact with other people, their ways of behaving and acting in these
social situations might simply work against them. For example, compared with less
shy individuals, shy people speak less, they take longer to respond to others’
dialogue, they have more difficulties in speaking their mind, they permit more silences
to develop in conversations, they are less likely to break these silences, and act more
inhibited and passive (Asendorpf, 1989; Borkovec, Fleischmann, & Caputo, 1973;
Cheek & Buss, 1981; Mandel & Shrauger, 1980; Natale, Entin, & Jaffe, 1979;
Paulsen, Bru, & Murberg, 2006; Pilkonis, 1977b; Prisbell, 1991). As research has
shown that simply talking to other people endorses being liked by others (Insko &
Wilson, 1977), it is perhaps no wonder that shy people do poorly in social
encounters. According to the self-presentational theory, people tend to feel shy when
they are stimulated to make a desired impression on other people but doubt that
they will be successful (Arkin, Lake, & Baumgardner, 1986; Leary & Buckley, 2000;
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 27
Schlenker & Leary, 1982). In these situations, shy people want to avoid possible
unwanted effects by acting inhibited, because inhibited behavior is a practical
reaction in situations where one is fearful of making an undesired impression (Leary
& Buckley, 2000; Schlenker & Leary, 1982). Thus, being shy and not talking to
others seems a sure path towards less social acceptance.
Third, individuals who are shy might come across as less appealing in terms of
attracting other people’s awareness and attention (Leary & Buckley, 2000). Other
people might not form the best opinions of shy individuals when interacting with
them. It is important to point out that it is not the case that others in general dislike
shy people. Not being able to communicate very well with others, however, has been
shown to be a nuisance for shy people who aspire to engage in social interactions.
Children who are quiet and less talkative are perceived as less socially competent and
less desirable as friends by their peers (Evans, 1993). Shy children differ from their
non-shy counterparts in that they tend to watch from the side when everyone else is
playing, they speak less than other children, and when they do speak they are slower
at initiating conversation (Asendorpf, 1990d). A similar pattern emerges in shy
children’s interactions with unfamiliar adults (Crozier, 2001). Peer-ratings show that
shy children are commonly viewed as less easy to approach and have low social
competence, making them less socially desirable than non-shy children (Evans,
1993). As a result, shy children can be treated more negatively by their peers (Blöte &
Westenberg, 2007). In adolescence, shy youths might have learned to avoid social
interactions, particularly in cases where they are unsure how to behave in an
appropriate manner (Crozier, 1979; Pilkonis, 1977b). As adults, shy individuals
might not know what to say in social circumstances, especially when it comes to
initiating conversations (Pilkonis, 1977b). They tend to sit more far away from
others during social situations, are often seen as less friendly and less assertive
towards others, and are distinguishable from non-shy people by independent
observers (Pilkonis, 1977b). In addition, shy people feel awkward or hesitant in
social interactions, are self-conscious, put too much effort into how they behave, and
might practice things to say beforehand (Crozier, 2000). Shy people might often
appear anxious to others, and will behave in a way that might reduce the potential of
social interactions by for example not speaking freely or speaking their mind
(Crozier, 2000). Hence, shyness is essentially connected to social experiences, and is
expressed in ways that can have consequences for those experiences (Crozier, 2001).
Compared with less shy people, shy individuals are less skilled at starting and
maintaining conversations, and they find it more difficult to demonstrate their
feelings and attitudes to others (Bruch et al., 1999; Miller, 1995). Shy people also
have problems with showing empathy and warmth when interacting with others, and
believe they have poorer skills to manage these interactions (Prisbell, 1991). The
negative features of shyness such as anxiety and awkwardness are related to how
much an individual is liked by people (Gough & Thorne, 1986). Thus, not appealing
to other people when interacting socially seems to be an additional problem for shy
people. Shyness, it seems, is easily identified by other individuals, and affects others’
views of shy people from childhood into adulthood.
28 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Situations that evoke shyness
There are several situations that might evoke the feelings of shyness for shy people.
In an attempt to find out what these were, Zimbardo and colleagues asked more
than five thousand individuals about their perceptions of situations they might shy
away from (Zimbardo, 1977). Among those individuals who reported being shy,
most stated that strangers made them shy, followed by the opposite sex, authorities
in knowledge, and authorities in virtue (Zimbardo, 1977). A smaller percentage of
people stated elderly people, friends, children, and parents to evoke such feelings
(Zimbardo, 1977). Regarding situations that make individuals shy, many were made
shy by being the center of attention in a large group, followed by being in large
groups, being of lower status, being in social and new situations in general, in
situations that require assertiveness, and being evaluated (Zimbardo, 1977). Almost
half of the participants felt shy when being the center of attention in a small group or
just being in small groups, in having one-to-one opposite sex interactions, and in
situations where they felt vulnerable or needed help (Zimbardo, 1977). Finally, a
third stated they felt shy in situations involving small task-oriented groups, and oneto-one same sex interactions (Zimbardo, 1977). Similar results have been found
elsewhere (Russell et al., 1986). Shyness might manifest itself differently in different
situations, nonetheless (Russell et al., 1986). For example, children who speak less in
unfamiliar situations might speak more in familiar settings (Asendorpf & Meier,
1993; Kagan, Reznick, Snidman, Gibbons, & Johnson, 1988). In addition, when shy
people hold a speech in an unstructured, novel experience, shyness has a larger
impact on the behavior in this situation then when being in a familiar setting
(Pilkonis, 1977b). Hence, different types of people and settings can evoke feelings of
shyness or social fears. In sum, shyness affects how individuals interrelate with
others in that it limits the prospect for social interactions, and makes people less
socially skilled and socially desirable.
Shyness and relationships in adolescence
Most of the research reviewed thus far has been on children or adults, including
university samples. A quick PsycInfo database search with the terms shy* and
adolesc* as keywords results in merely 173 studies in peer-reviewed journals, many
of which actually involve emerging adults or small children. In contrast, removing the
term adolesc* and searching only the term shy* results in 1427 hits. That means that
the studies that potentially include shy adolescents as a main focus of interest
amount to a maximum of twelve percent of the total amount of studies on shyness.
This lack of studies on adolescents is reflected when searching related terms to that of
shyness as well, such as social anxiety or social withdrawal. Surprisingly then,
research on shyness during adolescence is unexpectedly slim, even though there are
several reasons for studying this specific group.
One of the more important reasons to study youths is that adolescence is a
period of great transformation. As children make the transition from childhood into
early adolescence, they go through a large number of changes. First, they change
physically and hormonally, acquiring a more adult-like appearance (Buchanan,
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 29
Eccles, & Becker, 1992). Second, with these bodily changes come emotional and
cognitive adaptations that early adolescents must deal with (Damon, 1983). Third,
early adolescents’ roles in the society change; they have different expectations placed
upon them, with expanding social roles and changing relationships (Damon, 1983).
Their roles within the family change as well (Holmbeck, Paikoff, & Brooks-Gunn,
1995); youths start spending more time with peers and less with parents (Fuligni,
Eccles, Barber, & Clements, 2001). The significance of peers increases during early
adolescence (Bukowski, Gauze, Hoza, & Newcomb, 1991). Thus, there are many
new experiences for early adolescents. Novel occurrences of all kinds are a prominent
issue for shy individuals (Buss, 1980). It is then perhaps no wonder that early
adolescents might be specifically prone to shyness in this period of personality and
social development (Zimbardo, 1977). For that reason, it seems probable that the
large amount of changes and novelties linked with the transition into adulthood
might reinforce early adolescents’ shyness.
Another important reason to study adolescents, and especially early
adolescents, is because self-consciousness starts to arise during this phase of
development. For example, children have been found to be significantly less selfconscious than early adolescents (Simmons, Rosenberg, & Rosenberg, 1973). The
transition to junior high school might greatly contribute to this rise in selfconsciousness (Simmons et al., 1973). The occurrence of self-consciousness seems to
coincide with adolescent shyness, and scholars have argued that it is during late
childhood and adolescence that self-conscious shyness as such first appears (Buss,
1986). These findings also support the conception that novelty is the most influential
situational cause of shyness (Buss, 1980). Moreover, shyness that develops in
adolescence has been associated with poor romantic and sexual relationships, low
self-esteem, and low subjective wellbeing in middle adulthood, whereas this has not
been found for childhood shyness (Kerr, 2000). Consequently, it is of specific
interest to concentrate on this population when addressing social fears. Despite these
notions, however, childhood shyness has been the focus of a large amount of studies
on behavioral inhibition, social withdrawal, and social reticence, whereas a good
number of studies on social anxiety and shyness have employed adult, or university
samples. Hence, more spotlight on adolescents, and specifically early adolescents,
seems necessary for our understanding of the development of shyness and its
consequences across the lifespan.
Peer crowds. In adolescence, affiliating with peer crowds becomes very important,
perhaps more so than ever before. Young people start to identify and categorize
themselves and each other based on stereotypes and reputations. Such categories are
often referred to as peer crowds (Brown, Lohr, & Trujillo, 1990). Several peer
crowds have been identified in previous research, such as Jocks (athletically oriented),
Nerds and Brains (academically oriented), Populars (oriented towards social status),
Burnouts (normbreaking), Loners (youths who keep to themselves, social misfits),
and Alternatives (youths rebelling against social conventions). Youths belonging to
different peer crowds do not necessarily have to befriend or spend time with others
in the same crowd, but they might still feel they belong with a crowd (Urberg, 1992),
and can nonetheless be recognized by others as associated with that crowd (Stone &
30 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Brown, 1998). Where do shy youths fit into adolescent crowds? Are they without a
crowd, or do they belong to the crowds that are stereotypically associated with
shyness, such as Nerds and Brains? There are some hints about this in the literature.
For example, youths identify other peers who are not as “sociable” or “not with it”
as belonging to a Nobody crowd or to Brains, and these youths are reported to
spend time alone or with their family, and not with other peers at school (Brown et
al., 1990; Stone & Brown, 1998). However, these studies did not specifically focus
on shyness, and there are some characteristics of adolescent peer crowds that offer a
new way of thinking about where shy youths fit in and why.
One interesting aspect of peer crowds is that for the most part, membership is
not voluntary, but there are some crowds to which adolescents can intentionally
belong. Youths cannot choose by themselves whether they will be popular or not, for
instance, so being part of the Popular crowd is not entirely under their control.
Similarly, they might not want to be defined as a Nerd, but others might associate
them with that crowd anyway. Being identified as part of a more distinct peer crowd
such as a Punk or a Goth, on the other hand, means constructing a unique external
appearance—one that is easily identifiable with the crowd and easily distinguishable
from the norm. They might be called radical, because they often involve shocking
makeup and hair styles—white face paint, painted-on blood stains, blue or green
hair, for example. Without such an appearance, it would be improbable for a youth
to be recognized as belonging to crowds such as Punks and Goths. Hence, there
seems to be an intentional decision behind identifying with peer crowds that are well
known for their staggering or even shocking appearances. Would identifying with
such crowds be a way of coping with shyness for youths? As far as I know, this issue
has never been focused on in the literature on peer crowd identification nor shyness.
There are reasons to believe that assuming a shocking, even offensive style of
dress and makeup might be a means of coping for shy youths. This notion is based
on two theoretical reasons found in the literature on shyness. First, shy youths might
adopt a radical style in order to draw the social boundaries needed to alleviate
pressure and expectations of interacting with unfamiliar peers. Childhood studies
confirm that shy children find it difficult to interact with unfamiliar peers, as those
children have been found to talk as much as non-shy children do in familiar, but not
unfamiliar situations (Asendorpf & Meier, 1993; Kagan, Reznick, Snidman et al.,
1988). Being in a small, familiar group might create less anxiety for shy youths.
Along these lines, shy youths might also adopt radical styles in order to signal to the
social mainstream that they do not want any social contact, thus relieving themselves
of the pressure to socialize with others. A second explanation is based on the theory
of self-handicapping. This means doing something obvious to ensure one’s failure in
some domain so that if, or when, failure comes, it can be attributed to the action itself
as opposed to the person’s own abilities (Berglas & Jones, 1978). In this sense, shy
radicals might assign any possible embarrassment or negative responses by others to
their startling appearances, rather than to their shyness. Whether an alternative style
of dress might be a way for shy youths to cope with social fears, and how it might
affect their emotional adjustment, has been unanswered in previous literature.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 31
Problem behaviors with peers. Adolescents’ relationships with peers are positive in
many ways, but they are also connected to problem behaviors. What this would
mean for shy adolescents is not well understood. During early adolescence, problem
behaviors increase, and many adolescents become involved in behaviors such as
drinking alcohol, shoplifting, vandalism, and risky sexual activity. These acts usually
take place in the company of peers, and early adolescents are particularly susceptible
to peer pressure to engage in problem behavior (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005).
Shyness has been shown to reduce the risk of becoming involved in delinquency for
boys who were considered at risk because of their own disruptive behavior (Kerr,
Tremblay, Pagani, & Vitaro, 1997). Theoretically, this was attributed to a tendency
seen in shy, behaviorally inhibited children to hesitate for some time before taking
action in unfamiliar situations (e.g., (Kagan et al., 1989). The authors suggested that
the same tendency might inhibit action in the kinds of settings in which delinquent
acts take place. Such reasoning works well for delinquency, but it might not apply as
well for problem behaviors such as alcohol drinking and sexual activity. First, these
activities might take place in familiar settings with romantic partners, so the
behaviorally inhibited response would not necessarily be evoked. In early
adolescence, these problem behaviors are especially linked to advanced maturity
relative to peers—early pubertal timing or feeling more mature than peers—and there
is evidence that early adolescent girls get involved in these behaviors through
relationships with older boyfriends (Stattin & Magnusson, 1989). Second, there are
some indications in the literature that shy people might use alcohol to lower their
inhibitions (Hartman, 1986; O’Hare, 1990). Thus, the questions arise: (a) whether
shyness plays any moderating role in early adolescence for youths who are at risk of
engaging in drinking and risky sexual activity by virtue of early maturity and (b) if
so, what the nature of the moderation would be.
Regarding the link between shyness and alcohol use, previous research
suggests conflicting predictions. On the one hand, drinking alcohol is seen as a
potential means to reduce anxiety that comes with socializing (Burke & Stevens,
1999; Conger, 1956; Hartman, 1986; O’Hare, 1990). On the other hand, another
body of research demonstrates that shy individuals might come to refrain from
drinking alcohol because they fear losing control in social settings (Bruch et al., 1992;
Bruch, Rivet, Heimberg, & Levin, 1997; Dubow, Boxer, & Huesmann, 2008;
Eggleston, Woolaway-Bickel, & Schmidt, 2004; Ham & Hope, 2005; LaBrie,
Pedersen, Neighbors, & Hummer, 2008; Park, Sher, & Krull, 2006; Rogosch,
Chassin, & Sher, 1990; Rohsenow, 1983; Tran, Haaga, & Chambless, 1997). There
are several problems with this research, however, for the present purposes. First,
most studies are cross-sectional and as such do not reveal anything about the
processes involved. Second, most studies have been conducted using college students
or adults (Crawford & Novak, 2004; Eggleston et al., 2004; Ham, Bonin, & Hope,
2007; Kidorf & Lang, 1999; LaBrie et al., 2008; O’Hare, 1990; Park et al., 2006), so
the issues relevant in early adolescence—peer pressure, risks associated with early
maturity—are not present. For example, during early adolescence, shyness is linked
to low self-esteem (Cheek & Melchior, 1990; Crozier, 1995; Kemple, 1995; Schmidt
& Robinson, 1992; Smith & Betz, 2002), and youths with poor self-esteem are
especially vulnerable to negative peer pressures (Brown, 1990; Brown, Clasen, &
32 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Eicher, 1986). Some shy individuals adopt behavioral styles which can be described
as “going along to get along” (Cheek & Krasnoperova, 1999; Leary & Kowalski,
1995; Lewinsky, 1941) or “too shy to say no”, and may be characterized by
conform ideas and neutral attitudes. For shy youths, then, it might be particularly
difficult to say no to engaging in problem behaviors.
Shyness could also act as a buffer in reducing problem behaviors by restricting
youths’ chances to get romantically involved. Being in a steady relationship might act
as a gateway into engaging in more advanced behavior, particularly if the romantic
partner is older. Studies show that youths who are mature have older friends (Kerr,
Stattin, & Kiesner, 2007), and start having sex earlier than peers (Jessor, 1992). For
example, early adolescents might, through their boyfriend/girlfriend, start affiliating
with peer groups where having sex and drinking alcohol is common. In these cases,
shyness might prevent drinking or having sex in a number of ways. Research shows
that shy youths and adults go on fewer dates, have fewer sexual encounters, and are
less likely than non-shy people to be involved in a romantic relationship at any given
point in time (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998; Jones & Carpenter, 1986; Leary &
Dobbins, 1983; Prisbell, 1991; Zimbardo, 1977). When shy individuals conquer
their social fears and partake in social occasions, they do not talk as much with
others and tend to spend less time at such events (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998;
Dodge et al., 1987; Himadi et al., 1980; Twentyman & McFall, 1975; Watson &
Friend, 1969). Hence, shyness might constrain the prospects for youths to engage in
problem behaviors.
Drinking and having sex in early adolescence might not necessarily be
considered deviant, or even extreme, as much as it might for parents and other
adults. Many youths in Western societies today see intercourse and drinking alcohol
as rather normative behaviors (Vazsonyi, Trejos-Castillo, & Huang, 2006). Having
one-night stands or doing things one would regret after drinking are more extreme
variants of intercourse and drinking, as they involve a higher level of peril for youths.
On the one hand, if shyness might moderate, in one way or another, the link between
advanced maturity and problem behaviors, it might probably with more certainty
moderate behaviors of an even riskier nature. It might be that these types of
behaviors demand far too much disinhibition and impulsiveness than is generally not
part of a socially fearful behavioral profile. On the other hand, as shy individuals
have previously been found to adhere to conforming attitudes and behaviors (Cheek
& Krasnoperova, 1999; Leary & Kowalski, 1995; Lewinsky, 1941), perhaps they
might be more prone to engage in very risky behaviors after they would initiate more
“normative” levels of problem behaviors. These issues have, to my knowledge, not
been given any attention in the literature on shyness, and still remain unclear.
Peer selection and influence based on shyness. As humans are generally social
animals, relationships with others play a major role in most people’s existence.
People spend a lot of time together, and gather around holidays and celebrations in
order to be with family and friends. Indeed, to be entirely alone might be seen as a
harsh punishment in a society that so fully gravitates towards social relationships. In
middle childhood, more than a third of children’s social interactions starts to involve
people outside the family: friends and other peers (Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003).
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 33
By spending time with peers, children develop their self-definitions and self-esteem
(Bukowski et al., 1991), and learn to take other social perspectives and practice their
social skills (Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). In adolescence, youths spend more
time with peers than they do with their families (Bukowski et al., 1991; Fuligni et al.,
2001), making peer interactions increasingly important during this developmental
phase. Shy individuals, however, have much more difficulty with social contacts, and
their social fears obstruct smooth social relationships.
The knowledge about whether or in which way peers might socialize
adolescent shyness is scarce. Socialization might stem from two types of processes:
initial friendship selection, and friendship influence over time (Kandel, 1978). In the
case of shy youths, this means they would choose friends similar to themselves on
shy, socially fearful behaviors. As shyness is closely related to a desire for other’s
approval and fear of negative evaluation and rejection (Asendorpf, 1987; Jackson et
al., 1997; Jones et al., 1986; Leary & Kowalski, 1993; Miller, 1995; Pilkonis, 1977a;
Watson & Friend, 1969), it might be more simple for shy youths to seek out those
similar to themselves on shy characteristics. Some previous research on children has
viewed the issue of shyness as a criterion for friendship selection, and it was found
that those who were shy or socially withdrawn were indeed friends with similarly shy
children (Güroglu, Van Lieshout, Haselager, & Scholte, 2007; Haselager, Hartup,
Van Lieshout, & Riksen-Walraven, 1998; Rubin, Wojslawowicz, Rose-Krasnor,
Booth-LaForce, & Burgess, 2006). Same results have been found in middle
childhood (Haselager et al., 1998) and adolescence as well (Güroglu et al., 2007).
Similarity to friends can also be a result of over-time influence, however (Kandel,
1978). Shy friends might socialize one another into becoming more shy over time,
through several types of processes. They might for example extensively co-ruminate
their feelings and social problems, which might lead to avoiding social situations.
This type of process has been identified for adolescent girls’ depression (Prinstein,
Borelli, Cheah, Simon, & Aikins, 2005; Rose, 2002). Shy friends could also reinforce
one another by encouraging shy, avoidant behaviors, which might make them feel
better about their own social fears. Spending time with a similarly shy peer might
make it difficult to model efficient social interactions. To my knowledge, only one
study has viewed the influence of friends’ social withdrawal on one another, showing
that having a withdrawn friend when transiting to middle school increases social
withdrawal over time (Oh et al., 2008). Thus, whether or not friends in early
adolescence might socialize one another’s shyness over time is still unclear.
There are several empirical problems with the current research, as well. First,
shyness socialization has only been viewed in one study, including middle school
children (Oh et al., 2008). Thus, this has generally been uninvestigated in the
developmental literature. One reason for this might be that longitudinal data are
required for the study of influence (Kandel, 1978). Second, the study of socialization
poses demands on the ecological validity of the variables measured. One such
demand is to assess all relevant peers in a social network, including in and out of
school friends, and to get reports of shyness that are independent of the individual
who named the youths as friends. As youths tend to overrate how similar they are to
their friends (Furnham & Henderson, 1983; Morry, 2005), this aspect is of specific
consequence. One way to meet this empirical challenge is by including school grades
34 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
or classrooms in which everyone reports on their own behaviors. Including the
friends outside of school, and thus including the youths who do not have in-school
friends, is another. Third, a sound way of viewing friendships is from a larger
perspective, as embedded in social networks. Standard friendship studies limit
friendships to having participants name a best friend, thereby excluding other
friendships of a non-dyadic nature. Merely three studies on shy youths have been
conducted using a social network approach (Breidenstein-Cutspec & Goering, 1989;
Goering & Breidenstein-Cutspec, 1989, 1990). Whether friends might socialize one
another into becoming more shy over time was, however, not the focus of these
studies. Consequently, several questions regarding shyness socialization in early
adolescent friendships still remain.
Shyness and parenting . Concerning the relationships youths have with their
parents, not much is known about what influence parents have on the development
of youth shyness, as most empirical studies on the development and preservation of
shy behavior have concentrated on small children. Parental treatment of shy children
has been one of the most studied factors in this literature. Different types of socially
anxious behaviors in young children, such as shyness, behavioral inhibition, social
anxiety, reticence, and social withdrawal, have been linked with two kinds of parental
behavior: overcontrol and emotional coldness (Dadds & Barrett, 2001; Masia &
Morris, 1998; Rapee, 1997; Wood, McLeod, Sigman, Hwang, & Chu, 2003). It is
believed that parents who protect their children from challenging events or take
control in demanding situations might train their children to believe that the world is
an unsafe place from which they need protection and over which they have no
control (Rapee, 2001). Additionally, overcontrol might also hinder the development
of children’s self-regulation and feelings of self-efficacy and autonomy (Hastings,
Rubin, & DeRose, 2005; Mills & Rubin, 1998; Rubin et al., 2001; Rubin, Stewart,
& Chen, 1995), which might consequently make children’s shyness worse.
Concerning emotional coldness, the hypothetical idea is that as shy children are
growing up, if they recognize their parents’ behaviors as rejecting, they might grow
up to be preoccupied with others’ evaluative comments. This, in turn, may lead to a
general fear of negative evaluation, which is regarded as an important element of shy
behavior (Bruch, 1989). The idea is further that parents’ overcontrolling and
rejecting behaviors might, in that case, exacerbate their children’s shyness. Research
on small children partially confirms this notion, as studies have shown that mothers
of shy children are inclined to overcontrol and overprotect them (Hastings et al.,
2005; Rubin, Burgess, & Hastings, 2002; Rubin et al., 2001). In turn, mothers of
shy children lack warmth towards their children and tend to be cold or rejecting in
their manner (Grüner, Muris, & Merckelbach, 1999; Hudson & Rapee, 2001).
Thus, two types of parenting behaviors are known to have an effect on childhood
shyness.
The few studies conducted with adolescents have rested on parallel ideas, but
shyness has not been examined to the same degree as more generalized anxiety or
internalizing problems. For example, rejection and lack of warmth by parents appear
to enhance the risk of developing internalizing problems for early adolescents (Muris
& Merckelbach, 1998), and parental overcontrol and the absence of autonomy
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 35
encouragement is thought to contribute to anxiety disorders (Siqueland, Kendall, &
Steinberg, 1996). In addition, parents of anxious youths have also been found to
allow youths less personal independence, and youths report their parents as more
overcontrolling than do non-anxious youths (Siqueland et al., 1996). Anxious
youths report their parents as less warm, less supportive, and more rejecting than
non-anxious youths (Siqueland et al., 1996; Whaley, Pinto, & Sigman, 1999). It has
also been shown that the link between shyness and parental behaviors is
considerably weaker than the equivalent link involving anxiety (Van Brakel, Muris,
Bögels, & Thomassen, 2006). Hence, even though childhood shyness has been
strongly linked to parental behaviors, and adolescent anxiety has been linked to
similar facets of parenting, the relation between adolescent shyness and parenting
cannot be regarded as established.
Conversely, whether shyness might influence parental behavior as much as
parental behavior influences shyness has largely been uninvestigated. Suggestions
have been expressed in the literature regarding this link, as it has been shown that
small, temperamentally shy children most likely elicit different behavioral reactions
from their parents than children who are not as shy (Rubin & Mills, 1991).
Research on inhibition shows that inhibited children who get exposed to unknown
social circumstances are regularly more “difficult” and more easily aroused (Kagan,
Reznick, & Snidman, 1987). Parents might, in turn, find it difficult to calm and
comfort such children, and this might result in cold, rejecting behaviors from parents
(Rapee, 2001; Rubin & Mills, 1991). Similar suggestions have been made regarding
overcontrol, in that early shyness may induce overprotective or overcontrolling
reactions on behalf of parents (Rubin & Mills, 1991). Overprotection might be a
response to children’s social timidity and the anxiety that comes along with it (Rubin
et al., 2002; Rubin & Mills, 1990, 1991; Rubin et al., 1995). By being firm or
directive, parents might believe they are helping their shy child, but they might instead
help maintain or even worsen shyness (Rubin et al., 2002; Rubin & Mills, 1990,
1991; Rubin et al., 1995). Despite these theoretical ideas, however, few empirical
studies have investigated bidirectional links between parenting and shyness in
childhood and adolescence. In one study with children, for example, it was found
that shyness predicted a lack of encouragement from parents over two years, but not
the other way around (Rubin, Nelson, Hastings, & Asendorpf, 1999). In the only
longitudinal study including adolescents, the subject of attention was how the
associations relating social anxiety and parenting changed over the shift to the
teenage years, so the data were examined within time points, and prediction of
change across time was not attended to (Papini & Roggman, 1992). Consequently,
the question remains whether parents influence youth shyness, whether youth’s
shyness has an effect on parental behaviors toward their children, or both.
Summary
In sum then, even though many things are known about how shyness affects and is
affected by social relationships in childhood and adulthood, quite a few questions
remain unanswered about shyness and relationships in early adolescence. Those
regard different types of social relationships, such as those with peers, friends, and
36 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
parents. There are several features of early adolescence that make these questions
relevant. One is that in early adolescence peers begin to take on a more important
role than ever before. Early adolescents spend more time with peers than with their
families (Bukowski et al., 1991; Fuligni et al., 2001), and they increasingly confide in
their peers about intimate matters (Berndt & Perry, 1986; Buhrmester, 1990). They
start to become associated with reputation-based peer crowds, which are not entirely
of their own choosing, but which determine to a great extent how others see them.
Little is know about where shy youths might fit into these peer crowds—whether
they are typically left out, whether they belong to crowds that are stereotypically shy,
or whether they might intentionally choose to align themselves with peer crowds that
set them apart, socially, from the mainstream. Another feature of early adolescence is
that problem behaviors and pressures to conform to peers begin to develop, often in
combination (Allen, Porter, & McFarland, 2006). Some research suggests that shy
people have trouble standing up to others, as they often have poor self-esteem, which
in turn is highly linked with high susceptibility to peer pressure (Brown, 1990;
Brown et al., 1986). Problem behaviors that are associated with early maturation—
alcohol drinking and intercourse, as well as high-risk behaviors—are particularly
relevant for early adolescents, and the kinds of peer settings in which they take place
raise questions about whether and how shyness might moderate the risk of early
maturing adolescents engaging in these behaviors. On the one hand, shy youths
might be reluctant to be in party situations where drinking occurs, and they might
not engage in drinking for fear of losing control (Bruch et al., 1992; Bruch et al.,
1997; Eggleston et al., 2004). On the other hand, they might drink to lessen their
inhibitions (Hartman, 1986; O’Hare, 1990). Partly because having sex is often
linked to substance use, it might be affected in the same way. Another question about
peers derives from the phenomenon in early adolescence that selection of friends is,
more than in childhood, based on characteristics such as shared interests, opinions,
and intimacy, rather than more superficial features such as proximity and joint
activities (Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). This, together with findings that youths
select their friends based on similarity, raises the possibility that youths might select
friends based on similar levels of shyness. If so, then they might influence each other’s
shyness, as suggested by recent studies of depression. There is a small, recent
literature suggesting that girls increase each others’ depression levels through
discussing, or ruminating, together about their problems (Rose, 2002). It is
reasonable to think that shy youths might increase each others’ social fears by
talking and dwelling upon them together as well. Some evidence exists that shy
youths would choose friends similar to themselves (Güroglu et al., 2007), but
whether shy friends, in turn, might influence each other’s shyness over time has not
been thoroughly investigated. Finally, although peer relationships are important in
adolescence, parent’s social support also remains important (Rueger, Malecki, &
Demaray, 2008), but little is known about the possible role of parents’ behaviors in
the maintenance of adolescent shyness. Most studies on the subject have been carried
out with children, showing that parents tend to overcontrol shy children, and treat
them in an emotionally cold manner. Very few studies have explored the possibility
that shyness, in turn, might play an important role in this relationship as well. No
studies thus far have tested bidirectional links between adolescent shyness and
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 37
parenting behaviors. Thus, there are several unanswered questions regarding the role
of shyness in young people’s social worlds. These unanswered questions will be the
focus of this dissertation.
The aim of this dissertation
The main aim of this dissertation is to achieve a better understanding of how
adolescent shyness can affect and be affected by social relationships over time. The
dissertation consists of four studies. Study I offers an innovative theoretical
explanation to why shy youths might choose to affiliate with crowds that adopt offputting, startling appearances, and it also tests this idea. Study II tests whether
shyness can function as a protective factor in early adolescence against problem
behaviors such as risky drinking and risky sexual behaviors. Study III focuses on
shyness in a social network of youths, testing whether those who are shy prefer to
become friends with others who are shy. Additionally, it is also tested whether this
influences their shyness for the worse across time. Finally, Study IV examines the
bidirectional links between youths’ shyness and their perceptions of parenting,
testing parenting aspects such as overcontrol and emotional coldness. In addition, it
is also tested whether these processes differ for girls and boys. Moreover, differences
between the genders are tested regarding all of these processes. The following
research questions were presented: Focusing on the period of early adolescence,
1. How do people with whom adolescents have important relationships –
parents and peers – affect adolescents’ shyness, and how are these people, in
turn, affected by adolescent’s shyness? (Studies III and IV)
2. What does shyness mean for adolescents’ choices of relationships with peers?
(Studies I and III)
3. Do shy adolescents’ ways of dealing with peer interactions have consequences
for their internal and external adjustment? (Studies I and II)
4. Could shyness or youths’ ways of coping with it have positive impact on their
development? (Studies I and II)
38 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
I I M et hod
Participants and procedure
Sample 1
This sample has been used in Studies I, III, and IV. The data are from a 5-wave,
longitudinal study, conducted on community level in a city in central Sweden. The
first data collection took place in the fall of 2001, and the town population was
about 26,000 during this time. At the start of the longitudinal study, the city had
similar unemployment rates as the rest of the country (4%). The average income
(214.000 Swedish Crowns) was, however, lower compared to the rest of Sweden
(223.000 Swedish Crowns). In addition, 12% of the inhabitants in the town had a
foreign background. The main goals of the study were to understand the combined
roles of parents, peers, and individual characteristics in the development of
adolescent adjustment problems and delinquency.
All students in grades 4 through 10 (roughly, aged 10 to 18) were asked to
participate in the study each year. As one cohort of participants graduated high
school and left the study, another cohort of 4th graders came into the study each
year. Parents participated biannually, by filling out questionnaires we sent to them
and returning them in the mail. Only parents of 4th to 10th graders participated,
however, as youths attending 11th and higher grades would by Swedish standards
have reached the legal age of independence (18), or would be living on their own, or
both. As all the youths in the community were targeted, we were able to identify their
nominated friends, as these were likely involved in the study. In this way, the peers’
self-reported information about behaviors and relationships were available to us,
and were independent of the youths who named them, thus reducing the risk of
inflated similarity (Iannotti, Bush, & Weinfurt, 1996).
Youths were recruited in classrooms during school time. They were informed
about what kinds of questions would be part of the questionnaires, and how long it
would take to finish them. They were also told that participation was voluntary and
that they could do something else, should they choose not to take part. They were
guaranteed that if they did participate, their answers would not be revealed to
anyone else (for example, parents or teachers). Parents were informed about the
study beforehand in meetings held in the community and by a letter in the mail. With
the letter, they received a postage-paid card to return in case they did not want their
children to take part of the study (1% of the parents did so). They were also told
that they could withdraw their child from the study any time they pleased. Youths
filled out the questionnaires during regular school hours in sessions administered by
trained research assistants. Teachers were not present at that time. No one was paid
for participating, but for each of the classes in grades 4 to 6 we donated to the class
fund, and in each of the classes in grades 7 through 12 we held a drawing for movie
tickets. Everyone who stayed in the room, whether participating or not, was qualified
for the drawing. Overall youth participation rates were over 90% each year. The
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 39
procedures and measures were approved by the University’s Ethics Review Board at
the start and again at the mid-point of the longitudinal study.
In Studies I, III, and IV, the data used was starting from Wave 3 and onward,
as the shyness measure was available from that timepoint. In Study I, all 7th to 11th
graders (aged 13 through 18) at Wave 3 were included in the analyses. For Study III,
all 8th graders (age 14) at Wave 3 who had reported on their friends were included. In
Study IV, we included all the 7th–9th graders (aged 13 through 15) at Wave 3.
Sample 2
This sample was used in Study II. The data are from an ongoing longitudinal study,
with two waves of data collected thus far. The study began in the spring of 2007,
and two waves of data have been collected since. The study is conducted in a large
Swedish city, with a current population of about 131,200. The mean unemployment
rate in the city was 7.2% at the start of the study, which was somewhat higher than
the average unemployment rate of 6.1% in the entire country. The average income in
the municipality was 4% lower than the country average. In addition, 13.5% of the
inhabitants had an immigrant background. The primary purpose of the study was to
examine children’s views of harsh conditions in three domains: the family, the
neighborhood, and the school, and to assess how these three domains co-interact in
affecting children’s development. Schools known to be more successful with their
students, as well as schools known to have problems, were targeted in the study.
We recruited the youths in their classrooms during school time. We informed
them about the study, told them what kinds of questions they would be asked, and
how long it would take to fill out the questionnaires. The youths were informed that
their participation was voluntary, and if they did not want to participate, they could
withdraw at any time during the data collection. If they chose not to participate, they
were free to do something else instead. The youths were assured that their answers
would not be shown to anyone outside of the study, such as parents, teachers, or the
police. The parents were informed prior to the start of the study via letters, asking
for their consent. They were informed that they could withdraw their child from the
study at any time. Only 1% of the parents did not give consent for their children to
participate. Thus, youths took part in the study only if they and their parents wished
to do so. Participation rates were over 80% each year. The questionnaires were filled
out during regular school hours, and were administered by qualified research
assistants. Teachers were not present during the data collection, but were available in
case some students were problematic (which occasionally happened in the “problem”
schools). Youths were not paid for their participation. They received small gifts,
however, such as pens and calculators. For Study II, we used data from 7th–8th
graders (roughly aged 13 to 15 years) participating at the first wave of the study.
40 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Adjustment
Social relationships
Shyness and other
characteristics
Emotional adjustment
Depression (Radloff,
1977)
Self-esteem
(Rosenberg, 1979)
Peers
Self-perceptions of
crowd affiliation
Shyness (Gren-Landell et
al., 2009)
Behavioral inhibition
(Gren-Landell et al.,
2009)
Problem behaviors
Drunkenness
Intercourse
Risky drinking behaviors
One-night stands
Romantic Partners
Romantic involvement
Subjective age
(Galambos, Kolaric,
Sears, & Maggs, 1999)
Pubertal status (Williams
& Dunlop, 1999)
Shyness and maturity
Shyness
Table 1.
The measures used in Studies I through IV
Study I
Study II
Emotional adjustment
Depression (Radloff, 1977)
Friends
Friendship nominations (Kerr
et al., 2007)
Shyness (Gren-Landell et al.,
2009)
Shyness
Study III
Parents
Feeling overly
controlled by parents
(Kerr & Stattin,
2000)
Influence in family
decisions
Parental warmth
(Kerr & Stattin,
2003)
Parental coldnessrejection
Shyness (GrenLandell et al., 2009)
Shyness
Study IV
Measures
The measures used in the four studies are presented in Table 1.
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 41
Shyness and other characteristics
Shyness/Behavioral inhibition. For all the studies included in this dissertation, the same
measure of shyness was used. The measure consisted of 8 questions about fears in
social situations: speaking in front of the class, putting a hand up during class,
making a phone call to someone one does not know, being with classmates during
breaks, going to a party, initiating conversation with someone one does not know
very well, eating with others during lunch, and looking in someone’s eyes while
speaking. The participants rated themselves on a three-point scale, ranging from
having No fear (1) to A lot of fear (3) of the aforementioned situations. This measure
is part of a larger measure of social anxiety, first developed for adults (Furmark et
al., 1999), and later adapted for adolescents (Gren-Landell et al., 2009). The larger
measure contains measures of other aspects of social anxiety, including items about
being handicapped by social fears, which allows researchers to infer which people
meet the criteria for social phobia; however, all of the items included in the 8-item
scale I have used involve situations or behaviors similar to those that reliably
distinguish shy individuals from non-shy individuals, as they assess a wariness in
social situations (Cheek, Melchior et al., 1986). Thus, they capture the central feature
of shyness.
In Study I, The Cronbach’s alpha reliability for this scale was .75. In Study II,
the Cronbach’s alpha was .74. In Study III, the alphas were .75 for Time 1, .73 for
Time 2, and .75 for Time 3. The cross-year correlations ranged from .36 to .52.
Finally, for Study IV, the Cronbach’s alphas were .74 for Time 1, .72 for Time 2, and
.74 for Time 3. The cross-year correlations ranged from .48 to .64.
Maturity
In Study II, we assessed advanced maturity in two different ways. Primarily, we
measured girls’ and boys’ respective subjective age, to establish how old or young
youths perceived themselves to be relative to other peers. In addition, we asked girls
and boys independently about present pubertal status, to determine how biologically
developed they were.
Subjective age. In Study II, four questions about how boys and girls saw themselves
compared to peers their age measured subjective age (Galambos et al., 1999). The
first three questions pertained to feeling, looking, and being treated by other samesex peers according to one’s age, whereas the fourth question was about being
treated according to age by other-sex peers. The response items ranged from Much
Younger (1) to Much older (7). The alpha reliability for Time 1 was .89.
Pubertal status. We asked youths about the present state of their physical
characteristics in order to measure pubertal maturity (Williams & Dunlop, 1999).
Girls and boys were asked four separate questions, three of which were identical, and
one that differed between the genders. As the impending analyses in the study
required combining boys’ and girls’ answers into the same scale, the two differing
items were taken out. For both boys and girls, the three identical questions regarded
having grown in height very quickly, having body hair (e.g., pubic hair/armpit hair),
42 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
and experiencing skin change (e.g., oily skin and pimples). For girls, the fourth item
regarded breast growth, whereas boys were asked about voice change. Response
items ranged from No development (1) to Development completed (4). Cronbach’s
alpha for Time 1 for the three-item scale was .91, and .94 for the four-item scale. The
three-item scale correlated with the four-item scale at .94. Consequently, using the
three-item scale was considered appropriate.
Social Relationships
Peers
Self-perceptions of crowd affiliation. The measure of self-perceptions of crowd
affiliation was included in Study I. Information about crowds prior to assessing peer
crowd affiliation was gathered by talking to teachers, students, and youth leaders in
the community. These individuals reported on the types of crowds present. From
that information we developed a list of peer crowds. During the data collections,
youths were asked to choose from a list the peer crowd that they most identified
with. They were also given a choice to indicate that they did not identify with any
crowd, or they could write in a crowd if the one they identified with was not on the
list. From the crowds generated during the data collection, we selected a number of
crowds for a Radical group. For this group, we chose peer crowds that were
collectively considered by those who had spent time in the schools teaching and
gathering data as eye-catching in a way that seemed intended to shock, startle, or put
people off. Four crowds fit this description. Two of these, Punks and Goths, have
been documented in many previous studies of peer crowds. The other two might be
particular to Swedish youth culture, as they have, to our knowledge, not appeared in
previous research. The third crowd, Synths, were named for their interest in
electronically synthesized music, and they are recognized by severe, all-black clothing
and dyed-black, often dramatically styled hair. For example, half of the head might
be shaved and the dyed-black hair on the other half might be made to stand up like a
Viking horn. The fourth crowd, Aesthetics, are typically found among youths who
have chosen the music, drama, and art high school track (or among younger youths
who intend choosing this track later). They make startling appearances in a variety of
creative ways. They might adopt a Goth-like appearance with a lot of makeup used
by girls and boys alike; they might create space-alien-type sculptures of their hair;
they might put on welder’s goggles and vampire teeth and shave the hair off half of
their heads; or they might wear tails or other animal-like features. Thus, these four
crowds fitted our idea of a radical group, the so-called Radicals. We compared the
Radicals with three theoretically relevant crowd groups. The first one, Academics,
consisted of Computer Nerds and Brains, crowds which have previously been shown
to be shy (Kinney, 1993). Another comparison group, Independents, consisted of
youths who answered “None of the above [identities]” to the crowd question. We
chose to group these youths into the same category, as independents have been
found previously to have higher levels of social anxiety than youths in other typical
crowds (Prinstein & La Greca, 2002). Thus, our goal was to determine whether the
Radicals were more shy than these groups that have been identified previously as
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 43
inhibited, shy or socially anxious. The third comparison group, Non-Radicals,
comprised crowds that might be recognized by their clothing and hairstyles, but
which we did not consider Radical because their appearances were not shocking or
off-putting: “Sports,” “Role-Players,” “Vegans,” “Skaters,” “Teenyboppers,”
“Feminists,” “Poppers,” “Hip Hoppers,” “Ravers,” “Snobs,” and “Environmental
Activists.”
Romantic partners
Romantic involvement. We asked youths about their recent romantic relationship
status in Study II. The question was: Do you have or have you had a
boyfriend/girlfriend? The response items ranged from Have never had and don’t
want to have now (1) to Have now and have had before (5).
Friends
Friendship nominations. In Study III, youths were asked to identify up to three very
important peers, which we defined as “someone you talk with, hang out with, and
do things with” (Kerr et al., 2007). Youths were told that these should be very
important persons in their lives, but not their parents or other adults. We also
informed them that these important peers could be boys or girls, could live
anywhere, and did not have to be of the same age. In addition, the youths reported
on each important peer’s school and relationship (friend, sibling, or romantic
partner). Although siblings and romantic partners could be nominated, only friends
were included in the analyses for Study III. Thus, the friendship network in this study
consisted of up to 3 nominations of friends each participant considered important to
him or her.
Parents
We asked youths about their relationship with their parents in a number of ways,
and these measures were used in Study IV. First, we assessed perceptions of parents’
overly controlling behaviors. This included questions about feeling overly controlled
and perceiving a lack of influence in family decisions. Second, we used positive and
negative indicators of parental emotional coldness: parental warmth and
coldness/rejection. The aforementioned measures included separate reports about
mothers and fathers. These emotional reactions towards the adolescents were
regarded as likely to vary between mothers and fathers, whereas the overcontrol
measures were considered to reflect a family management tactic largely shared by
both parents.
Feeling overly controlled. In Study IV, five items measured whether youths felt overly
controlled by their parents (Kerr & Stattin, 2000). The items were: “Do you think
your parents give you enough freedom to do what you want during your free time,”
“Does it feel like your parents demand to know everything,” “Do you think your
parents control everything in your life,” “Do you think your parents butt into what
44 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
you do in your free time,” and “Do you feel like you can’t keep anything to yourself,
because your parents want to know everything?” The five-point scale ranged from
Yes, always (1) to No, never (5). The alpha reliabilities were .80 for Time 1, .82 for
Time 2, and .88 for Time 3. The cross-year correlations between the scales ranged
from .50 to .62.
Influence in family decisions. For Study IV, we used a scale that was developed in this
project. There were six items measuring how much influence the youths felt they had
at home, on a four-point scale ranging from Don’t agree at all (1) to Agree
completely (4). The items were “Your parents listen to you when decisions are to be
made in the family,” “You feel like you have influence and are partaking in things
that happen in your family,” “Your parents let you take part when you are going to
decide something in the family,” “If you have other points of view, then these
viewpoints can change decisions taken in the family,” “Your parents ask you when
decisions are to be made in the family,” and “When you are having a discussion at
home, you usually get to finish what you have to say.” The items were reversed, so
that higher score meant less influence (and thus more overcontrol). The alpha
reliabilities were .88 for Time 1, .88 for Time 2, and .89 for Time 3. The cross-year
correlations between the scales ranged from .44 to .59.
Warmth. Indicating parental emotional coldness in Study IV, youths were asked six
questions about how warm they perceived their mothers and fathers to be,
respectively (Kerr & Stattin, 2003). The items were “Your mom/Your dad”: “Praises
you for no special reason,” Shows he/she cares for you with words and gestures,”
“Does small things to make you feel special (e.g., winks, smiles),” “Constantly shows
how proud he/she is of you,” “Focuses on the positive and seldom on the negative
things you do,” and “Always shows his/her love to you without any reason – almost
regardless of what you do.” The response items ranged from Never (1) to Most often
(3) on a three-point scale. The alpha reliabilities for the items about mothers were .84
for Time 1, .85 for Time 2, and .87 for Time 3. The alpha reliabilities for the items
about fathers were .86 for Time 1, .86 for Time 2, and .88 for Time 3. The crossyear correlations ranged from .49 to .58 for the measures about mothers, and from
.48 to .55 for the measures about fathers. The correlations between fathers’ and
mothers’ scales ranged from .34 to .69.
Coldness-rejection. The youths were asked four questions in Study IV about their
mothers and fathers acting cold and rejecting towards them, and how their respective
parent typically reacted when they had done something they really did not like. The
items were part of a measure created in the project to tap parents’ negative and
positive responses to adolescents’ misconduct. The items were “Doesn’t talk to you
until after a long while,” “Is silent and cold towards you,” “Disregards your views
or ideas,” and “Avoids you”. The response items were on a three-point scale,
ranging from Never (1) to Most often (3). The alpha reliabilities for the scales
concerning mothers were .78 for Time 1, .75 for Time 2, and .81 for Time 3. The
alpha reliabilities for the scales concerning fathers were .79 for Time 1, .78 for Time
2, and .82 for Time 3. The cross-year correlations between the scales ranged from
.35 to .40 for the mothers’ measures, and from .40 to .42 for the fathers’ measures.
The correlations between the fathers’ and mothers’ scales ranged from .22 to .71.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 45
Adjustment
Emotional adjustment
Depression. In Studies I and III, we used the Child Depression Scale from the Center
for Epidemiological Studies (Radloff, 1977) to measure depressive symptoms. The
scale assesses symptoms such as worry, sadness, hopelessness, lethargy, and poor
appetite. The 20 items were rated on four-point scales from Not at all (1) to Often
(4). Youths were instructed to think about the past week. Examples of items are: I
have “Worried about things I don’t usually worry about,” “Felt scared,” “Felt down
and unhappy,” “Did not sleep as well as usual,” and “Felt lonely and without
friends.” The Cronbach’s alpha for this scale used in Study I was .90. The
Cronbach’s alphas for the scale used in Study III were .91 for Time 1, .92 for Time 2,
and .91 for Time 3. The cross-year correlations ranged from .51 to .60.
Self-esteem. Self-esteem was measured in Study I with a widely used scale (Rosenberg,
1979). The four-point response scale ranged from Not true at all (1) to Very true (4),
and the ten items assessed self-perceptions such as: “Considering all things, you are
pleased with yourself,” “You really feel worthless sometimes,” “You feel like you
have many good qualities”, “You wish you could think more highly of yourself,”
and “Sometimes you feel you are of no use.” The Cronbach’s alpha reliability was
.77.
Problematic Behaviors
In Study II, we assessed two types of problematic behaviors. The first set of
behaviors regarded drunkenness and intercourse - behaviors that, even though
adults might consider them as problematic, might not be seen that way by youths
themselves. The second set of questions regarded behaviors of a high-risk nature that
even youths might see as perilous, pertaining to risky behaviors after drinking and
having one-night stands.
Drunkenness. We asked youths’ about their drinking practices via one item. This item
was: “Have you had so much beer, liquor, or wine that you got drunk – during the
past year?” The five-item response scale ranged from No, it has not happened (1) to
More than 10 times (5). The cross-year correlation between the items at Times 1 and
2 was .62.
Intercourse. We assessed youths’ sexual activities with one item, which was: “Have
you had intercourse?” The response items ranged from No (1) to Several times (3).
The cross-year correlation between the two items at Times 1 and 2 was .61.
Risky drinking behaviors. To assess drinking behaviors of a more risky nature, youths
were asked about situations that had happened after they had been drinking. The
items were: “You don’t remember what you said nor did the day after,” “You ended
up in a fight or row,” “You said stupid things to others that you were ashamed of
afterwards,” “Your personality changed – you became a whole different person than
you usually are,” “You destroyed things such as windows, street lamps, phone
booths, furniture, benches, etc.”, and “You did other things that you regretted the
46 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
day after.” The three-item response scale ranged from No, it has not happened (1) to
Several times (3). The alpha reliabilities for this scale were .84 for Time 1, and .88 for
Time 2. The cross-year correlation between the scales was .56.
One-night stands. As further indication of high-risk behaviors, we assessed risky
sexual behaviors with one item. The item was: “If you have had intercourse, has it
happened that you’ve slept with someone on the first night?” The response items
ranged from Have not had intercourse (1) to Yes, it has happened several times (4).
The cross-year correlation between the two items was .59.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 47
I I I Resul t s
Study I
Previous research has shown that children view shyness as something negative (Fox,
Sobel, Calkins, & Cole, 1996) and young people actively try to deal with it by
adopting several coping strategies (Carducci, 2000). Among many different tactics
employed by shy persons to cope with their shyness, changing their appearance has
been identified as one (Carducci, 2000). Associating with peer crowds becomes
significant for young adolescents in this period of their lives. They start to think
about who they are, and categorize both themselves and other people based on
stereotypic images and reputations. Such categories, so-called peer crowds, have been
identified in previous research (Brown et al., 1990). Crowds such as athletically
oriented Jocks, academically oriented Nerds and Brains, socially accepted and
admired Populars, normbreaking Burnouts, socially isolated Loners, and rebellious
Alternatives have all been recognized as part of youth culture before. Even though
youths might not hang out or be friends with others in the same crowd, however,
they still might categorize themselves (Urberg, 1992) and be recognized by others
(Stone & Brown, 1998) as belonging to such a crowd. Youths might identify with
crowds on many grounds, such as sharing the same taste in music or enjoying
different kinds of sports. Affiliation with some crowds is, nonetheless, not something
one can necessarily be in charge of. Crowds such as Populars, for example, have
members that have risen in social status – a process not completely under one’s own
control. One can, however, choose to affiliate with a crowd such as Punks, as such
affiliation might imply constructing a distinctive outward appearance, and without
such an appearance it would be unlikely for a youth to be acknowledged as a Punk.
To our knowledge, the reasons for youths to identify with such peer crowds have
not been explored in the literature on peer crowd affiliation. We hypothesized that
identifying with crowds with off-putting appearances might be a way for shy youths
to deal with their shyness.
To determine whether such an alternative crowd, or what we term as Radicals,
were more shy than other crowd groups, we conducted planned comparisons with
Radicals as the reference group. They were compared with three theoretically valid
comparison groups: Independents, Academics, and Non-Radicals. The Radicals (M =
.42, SD = .89) were significantly more shy than the Independents (M = .09, SD = .93,
df = 3, p < .05), the Academics (M = .05, SD = 1.04, df = 3, p < .05), and the NonRadicals (M = -.13, SD = .98, df = 3, p < .001). These differences were true for boys
and girls alike as shown by a non-significant Gender x Crowd interaction (F = .22, df
= 3, p > .10). Academically oriented crowds resembling the Academics in this study
have been found to be highly shy in previous research (Kinney, 1993). We found the
Radicals to be even more shy than the Academics in our data. Thus, it seems that the
Radicals were most shy compared with the youths in the other crowd groups.
To further investigate the idea that adopting a Radical style serves as coping
strategy for shy youths, we reasoned that if adopting such an appearance and
manner of dress would be a successful strategy for shy individuals, then the shy
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 49
Radicals should be better adjusted than shy youths in the other crowd groups. To
examine this, youths who scored in the top 30% on the shyness measure were
categorized as highly shy (Kerr et al., 1997). Then, within this highly shy group, we
compared the Radicals (n = 26) with youths in the other crowd groups (99
Independents, 63 Academics, and 177 Non-Radicals). The results showed that the
most shy Radicals (M = 1.06, SD = .37) were not more shy than the most shy
Independents (M = 1.14, SD = .57, df = 3, p > .10), Academics (M = 1.21, SD = .65,
df = 3, p > .10), and Non-Radicals (M = 1.13, SD = .62, df = 3, p > .10). The shy
Radicals, however, felt significantly more depressed (M = .74, SD = .98) than shy
Academics (M = .22, SD = 1.01, df = 3, p < .05) and Non-Radicals (M = .24, SD =
.95, df = 3, p < .01). Additionally, the highly shy Radicals had significantly worse
self-esteem (M = -.85, SD = .77) than highly shy Independents (M = -.38; SD = .94, df
= 3, p < .05), Academics (M = -.25, SD = .88, df = 3, p < .05), and Non-Radicals (M
= -.28, SD = .84, df = 3, p < .05). Consequently, the shy Radicals did not differ from
other shy youths on their levels of shyness, but they were worse adjusted compared
with shy youths in the other crowd groups. We concluded that if some shy youths
are in fact adopting Radical appearances to cope with their shyness, their strategy
seems rather unsuccessful. Indeed, previous research has found some coping
strategies adopted by shy individuals as restricted in efficiency, or even
counterproductive (Carducci, 2000). Thus, these results are in accord with earlier
findings.
Study I provided additional knowledge about shy early adolescents’ potential
choices of social identities. These youths do not merely spend time with peers,
however. Trying out new things, getting romantically involved, or attending typical
youth social events such as parties often characterize the teenage years. These
characteristic adolescent activities do not always have positive consequences,
nonetheless, and might lead the way to problem behaviors. What influence might
shyness have during this period in people’s lives? Study II focuses on this issue.
Study II
In Study I, it was shown that shy youths were willing to take to extreme measures in
order to fit in somewhere. This is, perhaps, no wonder, as early adolescence is
inevitably a period of turmoil for young people, for several reasons. Youths go
through puberty and physically change to resemble adults. Many youths do this
seemingly over night, and suddenly become all “grown up”. Whether or not they act
mature, however, can depend on how old they feel in relation to others their age, and
how far along their pubertal maturity they are. That is, youths’ advanced maturity
plays a role in their development as well. More advanced maturity has previously
been linked with many negative outcomes, as youths who feel older and are more
pubertally advanced than their same age mates get involved in problem behaviors
more often than youths whose maturity either matches their actual age, or who are
late maturers (Galambos et al., 1999). Another important part of early adolescence is
the increased significance of friends. Problem behaviors are often initiated with
friends, however. Typical problem behaviors during early adolescence are for
50 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
example drinking and engaging in sexual activities. Such activities, nonetheless, often
require presence of others, and can be more easily initiated when youths attend
parties and other social get-togethers, or if they are romantically involved, for
example. As was illustrated in Study I, shy youths were more likely to choose an
alternative social identity, perhaps as means of coping with their shyness. This
strategy was not successful, however, possibly because with these styles might come a
certain level of impulsivity or disinhibition that shy youths would not find
comfortable. For example, drinking a lot or having sex might be problematic for very
shy early adolescents. We wondered if shyness, in that case, might serve as a
protective factor for problem behaviors such as drinking and intercourse, but also
high-risk behaviors such as risky sexual pursuits and risky drinking behaviors over
time – despite youths’ advanced maturity. We also wanted to find out whether this
would differ for boys and girls.
In Study II, youths were assessed for two consecutive years. We first examined
the assumption that advanced physical or subjective maturity might act as a risky
condition for problem and risky behaviors. Second, we wanted to know whether one
type of problem behavior (e.g., intercourse) would lead to another (e.g., drinking),
and the other way around (see Figure 1, Study II). Using structural equation
modeling, we found that advanced maturity, as defined by subjective age and
pubertal status, was related to both drunkenness and intercourse. These indicators
of advanced maturity were also linked to each other. Moreover, problem behaviors
seemed to predict one another over time (Chi2 = 15.27; df = 4; p < .05; RMSEA =
.06; CFI = .99). Similar patterns were found when using high-risk indicators of
problem behaviors in the model (Chi2 = 21.33; df = 4; p < .001; RMSEA = .07; CFI =
.98). The difference was that pubertal status, in this case, was not predictive of
engaging in one-night stands, and was only marginally linked to risky drinking
behaviors. Even in the case of the high-risk behaviors, however, engaging in one
high-risk behavior seemed to lead to engaging in the other, and the other way
around. Our questions were then directed to whether, and how, these links might be
moderated by shyness. That is, could shyness serve as a protective factor on the links
between advanced maturity and problem behaviors?
As a second step, we examined the possible moderating effects of shyness, as
well as gender, on the link between advanced maturity and problem behaviors. After
dividing the shyness variable by the median (the lower 50% were non-shy, the top
50% shy), and further dividing it by gender, we thus compared four groups: nonshy girls, shy girls, non-shy boys and shy boys (see Figure 2, Study II). We conducted
multiple group differences in MPlus to ascertain whether these groups differed on the
different links in the model described previously. Shyness was protective for girls on
the link between subjective age and having intercourse. Advanced pubertal status at
Time 1 was predictive of drunkenness for non-shy boys, whereas this link was not
significant for the three other groups. Shyness was also protective on the link
between drunkenness at Time 1 and intercourse at Time 2, in this case only for boys.
In the second set of analyses using risky drinking and one-night stands as indicators
of high-risk behaviors, shyness was mostly a protective factor for boys, and non-shy
boys seemed to be in the danger zone regarding most of the links (see Figure 3, Study
II). The link between pubertal status and subjective age at Time 1 and one-night
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 51
stands, as well as the link between pubertal status and risky drinking was moderated
by shyness for boys. Time 1 risky drinking was also a significant predictor of
increase in having one-night stands at Time 2 for non-shy and shy boys, although
the link was the strongest for non-shy boys. In addition, having one-night stands at
Time 1 was also a significant predictor of risky drinking at Time 2 for non-shy boys,
and shyness was thus protective for boys. Thus, it seems that shyness as a protective
characteristic plays a more central role in the relation between advanced maturity and
high-risk behaviors for boys.
In the third and final step, we wondered whether taking into consideration
youths romantic involvement might in some way alter or diminish the effects of
shyness and gender on the association between advanced maturity and problem
behaviors. That is, if youths are of an advanced age, and are romantically involved,
this might serve as a gateway into social networks where drinking and having sex
might be more normative, or even expected, of youths. Thus, the protective effects of
shyness might be diminished. To test this idea, we conducted the same models as
described previously, only we also included romantic involvement as control at Time
1 (see Figure 4, Study II). As one would expect, romantic involvement was in itself a
strong predictor of both problem behaviors, as well as high-risk behaviors. Overall,
however, the results did not support the idea that controlling for romantic
involvement would diminish the protective role of shyness. On the contrary, the
results in the conducted models showed very similar patterns to those found
previously. For problem behaviors, shyness was still protective regarding similar
links for both boys and girls. And for high-risk behaviors, like previously, shyness
was mainly protective for boys (see Figure 5, Study II). In sum then, the findings
from Study II supported moderating effects of shyness on the link between advanced
maturity and various problematic behaviors, although the findings generally varied
for boys and girls. The patterns of results were overall quite similar to those with
problem behaviors, even after controlling for adolescents’ romantic involvement. We
concluded that shyness, in combination with gender, was indeed a protective factor
for problem behaviors in early adolescence regardless of subjective age, pubertal
status, or participant’s romantic involvement.
It seems then, that shyness as a characteristic might be protective against
problem behaviors. Hence, shyness might serve as a buffer against being socialized
into typical youth problem behaviors during early adolescence, which potentially
might lead to more problems later on in life. What is not so well-known, however, is
how shyness in itself might be socialized by friends and peers. What are important
characteristics of friends that shy youths choose? And might their shyness influence
these friendships in some way? These and additional questions were attended to in
Study III.
Study III
Study II investigated whether shyness might be protective for the socialization of
problem behaviors in early adolescence. Study III, on the other hand, focused on the
socialization of shyness itself. Peers and friends start to play an increasingly
52 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
important role in early adolescence, as youths spend less time with family and more
with friends. Whether or how friends’ might socialize or influence each other’s
shyness has, to our knowledge, not received much focus in previous research.
Socialization can be the result of two types of processes, however. First, youths can
select one another as friends on the basis of for example interests, behavioral
characteristics, attitudes, and so forth. Several previous studies on children, and one
study on adolescents, report that those with shy, socially withdrawn characteristics
tend to choose similar friends. What impact choosing a shy friend might have on shy
adolescents over time, however, still remains unclear. The study of socialization,
however, requires ecologically valid assessments of friends. Studying classroom peers
or limiting friends to reciprocated best friendships generates problems. Assessing all
relevant peers in a social network, both in and out of school, and getting
independent reports of shyness are crucial in order to meet such challenges. All
possible friends, regardless of ages, gender, or being in and outside of school, should
be incorporated so as to achieve the most ecologically sound way of measuring youth
friendships. Finally, viewing friendships embedded in a larger network allows a better
look at the whole picture. Indeed, friendships are not merely dyadic, and being a
friend of a friends’ friend is common. Thus, all of these factors should be taken into
consideration, to be able view socialization processes in a suitable way.
As a first step, we conducted our analyses using recently developed software
called SIENA (Simulation Investigation for Empirical Network Analyses; Snijders,
Steglich, Schweinberger, & Huisman, 2007), which allowed for following the
development of friendships in a social network of early adolescents. The advantage
of SIENA is that it controls for various selection effects in friendships. In our study,
we chose gender and depressive symptoms, but also being a friend of a friend (i.e.,
associated in a triadic relationship). We followed the development of youths’
friendships three years in a row. Overall results for all adolescents in the study
showed that the youths in the social network had a tendency to keep the friends they
already had, as opposed to acquiring new ones. Additionally, around sixty percent
of youths at each of the three time points in our network had mutual dyadic
friendships. Almost forty percent of the youths had relationships that involved triads
(or being a friend of a friend) at Times 1 and 2, with a slight decrease at Time 3.
As a second step, we wondered whether shyness as a characteristic might be a
motivation for choosing other shy friends. As the results from this study illustrated,
shy youths in our study had a tendency to select others who are shy as friends, they
were selected less by others, and selected fewer friends themselves over time. That is,
regarding the selection effects of shyness on friendships on the one hand, we found
that shy youths nominated fewer friends, and they also had a lower tendency to be
nominated as friends in the network. Hence, as would be expected, shy youths did
not have as many friends as their less shy peers, and they were not as popular in the
network. Finally, we found that shy youths had a tendency to choose other shy
youths as friends. We also found some interesting gender effects. For example, girls
had a tendency to nominate friends more than boys. Girls, however, also had a lower
tendency to be nominated as friends in the network. Or, put differently, girls were
more active in the network than boys, but were not as popular as boys. Boys had a
tendency to nominate boys, and girls had a tendency to nominate girls as friends in
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 53
the network. Regarding the socialization effects of shyness on friendships, we found
that friends tended to influence each other’s shyness over time. Hence, if shy youths
chose shy friends, they became more shy over time. In addition, we found the girls to
be influenced more than boys by their friends’ shyness. All of these aforementioned
results were true over and above other selection and influence effects, such as gender,
depressive symptoms, and the tendency to form triadic relationships.
Thus, as Study III showed, shyness as a behavioral characteristic can certainly
affect youths’ choices of friends, which in turn have shown to be socially unfavorable
in terms of making them more shy over time. As we have shown thus far, being shy
might alter one’s social identity (Study I), might serve as a protective factor for
problem behaviors (Study II), and might change the way youths function in a larger
social network of people. In all of the abovementioned studies, we showed that shy
behaviors could ultimately change the way youths interact with their surrounding
social worlds, in both positive and negative ways. Even though early adolescents
might share increasingly large parts of their social worlds with peers and friends,
however, their world outside the one with friends is most likely shared with parents.
How youths’ shyness might impact parenting or the other way around, however, is
not clearly understood. The fourth and final study addresses this issue.
Study IV
In the previous studies, we showed that early adolescents’ shyness changed their
relationships with peers and friends. It is inevitable that young people share their
social worlds with others, and these others will influence how they feel about
themselves. Youths can, to a certain extent, choose crowds to identify with, and they
can certainly select the friends they want to spend time with. They cannot, however,
choose their parents. How parents’ treatment might affect shyness during early
adolescence is, nonetheless, not so well understood. There are several theories on
how parenting might impact shyness. These theories have mostly been applied in
studies examining the link between parenting and shyness in childhood. For
example, research shows that mothers of shy, anxious children tend to overcontrol
them and act in a cold, rejecting, manner towards their children (Dadds & Barrett,
2001; Masia & Morris, 1998; Wood et al., 2003). Not much is known about
whether this association is still present in adolescence, and whether shyness, in turn,
might impact parenting as well. Testing such associations, however, demands the use
of longitudinal data. Study IV is among the few studies that has employed such data
in order to explore bidirectional effects on the links between shyness and parenting
in early adolescence.
In Study IV we tested the mutual effects between youths’ shyness on the one
hand, and parental overcontrol and emotional coldness on the other hand. We used
longitudinal data over three subsequent years. Youths reported on shyness and three
types of parenting behaviors often linked with shyness in childhood studies: feeling
overly controlled by parents, parental warmth, and parental coldness-rejection (see
Figure 1, Study IV). We found that youths’ shyness at Time 1 predicted an increase
in feeling overly controlled, lack of parental warmth, and coldness-rejection at Time 2
54 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
(Chi2 = 870.39; df = 149; p < .0001; RMSEA = .07; CFI = .91). Reversely, feeling
overly controlled at Time 2 predicted shyness at Time 3. We also found that shyness
at Time 2 predicted an increase in lack of parental warmth at Time 3. On the other
hand, lack of parental warmth at Time 1 predicted shyness at Time 2, albeit this
effect was marginally significant. Overall then, there was some support for
bidirectional effects between shyness and parenting across time. These pertained
primarily to overcontrol and parental warmth. Regarding most of the significant
paths in the model, however, youths’ shyness seemed to be the driving force in their
relationship with their parents. In addition, the strongest link in our model was
between shyness and coldness-rejection, showing that shyness might evoke cold,
rejecting responses from parents more vividly compared with overcontrol and lack of
warmth. Finally, we did not find differences between girls and boys on this model
(Chi2difference = 13.63; dfdifference = 14; p > .10). Thus, we concluded that the processes of
shyness impacting parents’ behaviors, and parents’ behaviors reversely impacting
parenting were not different for boys and girls. Overall then, the effects of shyness on
early adolescents’ social worlds might be detrimental in some ways (Study I, III, and
IV), but not necessarily regarding all aspects of development (Study II). To be sure,
shyness could also act as a buffer against problem behaviors during this particular
phase of life.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 55
I V Di scussi on
Findings and previous research
“The world doesn’t understand me and I don’t understand the world, that’s why
I’ve withdrawn from it”.
Paul Cezanne, painter
The primary purpose of this dissertation is to gain a clearer knowledge about how
adolescent shyness affects, and is affected by social interactions, situations, and
relationships, which are in this dissertation jointly referred to as social worlds. An
additional focus was the influence of shyness on adolescent emotional adjustment. In
general, it has been found that shyness in early adolescence can change social worlds
and impact youths’ emotional adjustment in a number of unconstructive ways.
Shyness can, however, also play an important protective role in that it can shield
adolescents from getting involved in problem behaviors. One of the core messages
from all of the findings taken together is that shy adolescents are active agents in
their own lives. They are not merely submissive partakers of what their behavioral
characteristics might evoke in others around them. They try to cope with their
shyness, whether or not these attempts are successful. Contrary to common belief,
their shyness is an important driving force in the relationships with parents, and not
the other way around. They choose their friends the way they see fit. Ultimately, shy
individuals are not passive recipients of whatever the world throws at them; they
interact with their social worlds in the manners they know how. Indeed, some
previous research shows that shyness in itself might even be used as a strategy to
avoid things that seem unpleasant, and in that way shyness might not only be a
problem – but also a solution in itself (Snyder & Smith, 1986). That is not to say
that shy youths are not affected by the world around them, as we also have shown. It
is merely stating that being shy does not necessarily mean fulfilling the prophecy of
being the nervous, friendless person in the back of the classroom with nothing to
say.
This dissertation adds to the knowledge about early adolescents’ shyness and
its effect on social worlds in several significant ways. Most importantly, it portrays
shy youths as active agents in social relationships. First, the findings from this
dissertation further clarify the link between shyness and parenting in early
adolescence. Previous research has focused on childhood, showing that for example
mothers of shy, anxious children tend to overcontrol them, (Hastings et al., 2005;
Rubin et al., 2002; Rubin et al., 2001), and act in a cold, rejecting manner (Grüner et
al., 1999; Hudson & Rapee, 2001). The findings in this dissertation demonstrate,
however, that adolescent shyness might have more impact on parents’ behaviors
than was previously assumed. Indeed, it was hypothesized that in adolescence, the
relationships with parents might be the main driving force in maintaining or
exacerbating youths’ shyness. Some evidence for bidirectional links, however, was
also found, in that some aspects of parenting, such as overcontrol, also influenced
youths’ shyness for the worse. The results confirm, nonetheless, that in this period in
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 57
their lives, youths’ shyness is a prominent characteristic, influencing relationships
with others. Whereas shy children are inevitably more under the control of their
parents, early adolescents are seeking more and more independence. This
dissertation, thus, adds, to the current growing view of youths being portrayed as
active agents in their relationships with parents. In addition to these findings, this
dissertation shows that shy youths seek out others who are shy, making them less
compliant in friendships than one might think. We found that shy youths’ friendship
selection had a tendency to, either consciously or unconsciously, largely be based on
others’ socially bashful characteristics. We hypothesized that perhaps in this way, shy
youths can “be themselves” and feel relaxed from the otherwise overwhelming social
pressures from other, non-shy people. Thus, this dissertation points to the
importance of studying shy youths over time, embedded in social networks, and
taking into account both their social timidity alongside others’ responses to such
characteristics.
Consequently, the current dissertation goes hand in hand with contemporary
theories about transactional processes in social relationships. As children are
growing up, they will progressively develop and operate on a variety of short-term
and long-term goals that influence their own socialization and their relationships
with parents (Kuczynski, 2003; Kuczynski & Parkin, 2006). Children are, in this
sense, active agents in their own socialization. Likewise, parents partake in their own
socialization processes together with children (Kuczynski, 2003; Kuczynski &
Parkin, 2006). Hence, instead of contributing socialization processes to specific
behaviors, traits, or variables, the relationships between children and parents are
transactional by nature in that they comprise mutual exchanges between parents and
children across time (Kuczynski, 2003; Sameroff, 1975). Or put differently, parents
affect their children and children affect their parents, and together they partake in
each other’s socialization and development. This same idea could most likely be
applied to youths’ relationships with their friends. As was shown in Study III, friends
influence each other’s shyness over time. They select each other on certain bases, and
in doing so they will inevitably partake in one another’s socialization processes.
Another contribution of this dissertation is that it yields more understanding about
shy youths surrounded by a larger social network of people. Even though previous
studies of shy individuals’ social interactions have offered insights about how they
function under such conditions (Crozier, 1979; Pilkonis, 1977b), very little research
has attempted to view shy youths from a bird perspective (Breidenstein-Cutspec &
Goering, 1989; Goering & Breidenstein-Cutspec, 1989, 1990). Additional literature
has examined the characteristics of shy individuals’ friends (Schneider, 1999), with
the conclusion that they tend to be shy. Former research has, nonetheless, seldom
followed shy individuals over time, thus making it impossible to draw conclusions
about directions of effects. For such attempts to be made paired with a social
network approach, large amounts of data gathered over time are necessary. In this
dissertation, we have showed that shy youths have fewer friends, they tend to select
friends who also are shy, and they influence each other’s shyness over time for the
worse. By using a novel approach while studying social networks, this dissertation
makes a unique contribution to the literature on shy youths’ social worlds. Overall
58 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
then, the results from this dissertation maintain the notion about transactional
influences in shy youths’ social relationships.
This dissertation also expands the knowledge about the impact of shyness on
adolescent adjustment. For example, shyness can impact social worlds in some
beneficial ways as well. Indeed, focusing on positive and not only negative aspects of
shyness is far more customary in research of the subject matter (Pilkonis &
Zimbardo, 1979). This dissertation has studied problem behaviors such as alcohol
use and engaging in sexual activities in early adolescence. Previous research has
found that shyness can serve as a barrier (Bruch et al., 1992; Dubow et al., 2008;
Ham & Hope, 2005; LaBrie et al., 2008; Park et al., 2006; Rogosch et al., 1990;
Rohsenow, 1983; Tran et al., 1997), but also as incentive (Burke & Stevens, 1999;
Hartman, 1986; Lewis & O’Neill, 2000; O’Hare, 1990) for drinking alcohol. In
addition, studies of sexual activities have shown similar results, in that non-shy
individuals were found to engage in sexual activities earlier and more frequently
(Udry, Kovenock, Morris, & Van den Berg, 1995). As the results in this dissertation
exemplify, shyness in early adolescence can serve as protective factor, in that it can
buffer young people from problem as well as high-risk behaviors. Thus, this
dissertation highlights certain positive facets of shyness that might not come across
as quite so apparent when considering its role in youth adjustment.
Another key contribution regarding the impact of shyness on youth
adjustment is that shy youths’ attempts to cope with their social wariness might
result in adopting a social identity, which, to say the least, is out of the ordinary. This
finding might seem counterintuitive at first, as previous research on shyness has
linked shy behaviors with clichéd interactions where shy individuals say bizarre
things out of nervousness, sit on their own, have trouble looking into other people’s
eyes, and act socially inept in general (Pilkonis, 1977b). Shy individuals are thought
of as persons that don’t draw too much attention to themselves in any noteworthy
way. The findings in this dissertation illuminate, nonetheless, that this might not
necessarily be the case. The shy youths in our data attempted to cope with their
shyness in the most colorful of ways, making themselves decidedly noticeable to
others, and in that way perhaps scaring off new potential social contacts. Indeed,
most of us might feel hesitant in approaching a person with an appearance so very
far from the social norms; an appearance that might signal that the person wants no
communication with others. Needless to say, shyness is not automatically the only
driving force in this progression. Even if shyness might be very present as a
characteristic, there are inevitably other things that might impact youths’ choices of
social identities. From the point of view that shyness is a stable characteristic or a
trait, however, this dissertation shows that it will unavoidably steer individuals’
social lives in certain directions.
Finally, little is known about shyness in adolescence. For the most part,
studies involving shyness have employed either children or adults. In many cases, the
participants have been university students. There are several theoretical reasons that
make adolescents a population of interest. Adolescents go through physical changes
that make them look and feel more mature (Buchanan et al., 1992). They also go
through emotional and cognitive adjustment they need to deal with at the same time
(Damon, 1983). In addition to these changes, adolescents’ societal roles change
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 59
drastically, as people around them might expect different things compared to
children; their social roles develop and relationships change, both in general (Damon,
1983) and within their family (Holmbeck, Paikoff, & Brooks-Gunn, 1995). Thus, it
seems that early adolescents experience many new things. Such novel experiences of
different sorts are a well-known problem for individuals who are shy (Buss, 1980). It
has been suggested that it is therefore conceivably no wonder that shy youths might
be specifically prone to develop social fears during this period of their lives
(Zimbardo, 1977). Studies show differences regarding several aspects of emotional
adjustment between childhood and adolescent shyness, where later-developing
shyness seems to have more detrimental effects (Kerr, 2000). The research on
shyness in adolescence, however, has been rather slim in the literature compared with
childhood and adult studies.
Strengths and limitations
This dissertation has certain limitations that warrant recognition. One important
limitation pertains to generally over-estimating the direction of effects in the studies.
All individuals are embedded in different development periods of their lives. In this
dissertation, we have only followed early adolescents for a short number of years,
and can thus generalize these findings to this specific phase in their lives. Accordingly,
what ever happened previously in childhood, and what might result later on from the
interactions we followed during early adolescence is difficult to say anything about.
In addition, these youths have preceding histories of social interactions with their
parents and friends. What has occurred in these interactions prior to our assessment
of the youths also remains unknown. We have, nonetheless, found effects of shyness
on social relationships during the period of early adolescence that are of directional,
and in certain cases bidirectional nature.
In addition, we are not able to account for all kinds of social circumstances
that the early adolescents in our data might have been, or still are, subjected to in
their lives. All people partake in their own realities, surrounded by specific contextual
factors such as the types of neighborhoods they live in, whether they have wealthy or
poor parents, or what the society around them looks like and what kinds of
demands it poses on individuals. Narrower social contexts are, in turn, the youths’
family. Their relationships with parents, siblings, relatives, and other significant
persons were not automatically taken into account in all of the studies in this
dissertation, nor have we observed how these youths actually function in their
everyday lives. All we have is their own narratives of their own lives, their personal
statements in the form of questionnaires. Nonetheless, these personal stories might
yield insight into their lives that no observation or experiment might. Accordingly,
this dissertation has assessed and illustrated the impact of shyness on early
adolescents social worlds in a way that provides us with new information about the
trials and tribulations of being shy in a world that promotes contrasting qualities.
Regarding the individual studies in the dissertation, there are some
overlapping limitations that ought to be mentioned. First, the lack of other
observers’ reports of shy youths’ behaviors is a limitation in almost all studies.
Parents, teachers, friends, and other adults might have provided useful facts on shy
60 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
youths’ behaviors, and helped to avoid the issue of informant bias. Such information
was, nonetheless, unavailable to us. Some scholars have argued, however, that
youths’ own reports of their relationships might be the most accurate, compared
with other observers, because for example parents’ reports often contradict youths’
reports (Sessa, Avenevoli, Steinberg, & Morris, 2001). Others have argued that
parental information might not even be valid for descriptions of children’s
behavioral characteristics (Kagan, Snidman, McManis, Woodward, & Hardway,
2002; Seifer, Sameroff, Barrett, & Krafchuk, 1994). It has also been shown that
children might sometimes be better reporters of what goes on inside the family
compared with their parents, as the children’s own reports have been found to
correlate more highly with other observer’s reports of parental behaviors than the
parent’s own reports (Sessa et al., 2001). Thus, even though it would have been ideal
to have other-observer reports on the youths in our studies, we have tried dealing
with informant bias in certain ways, under the circumstances of the nature of our
data. For example, we have included reciprocated friend nominations in one study.
In addition, sometimes we had youths’ reports available separately for parents,
which might have added more information than would be available with reports on
both parents simultaneously. In general, even though our data might mainly be
representing youths’ own perceptions of what is going on in their lives, the
consistency of the findings across the studies shows that our results might indeed be
taking into account genuine developmental processes in the lives of shy youths.
Finally, a limitation not dealt with in the studies overall, is the lack of
distinction between childhood and adolescent shyness. As both of the samples used
in the dissertation comprised early adolescents, it is difficult to know whether these
youths were already shy as children, or whether their shyness started blooming as a
result of self-conscious concerns that arise during late childhood and early
adolescence (Bruch et al., 1986; Buss, 1980; Buss, 1986; Schmidt & Robinson,
1992), possibly intensified by the transformations that take place throughout
puberty (Cheek, Carpentieri et al., 1986). As adolescent shyness has, in comparison
with childhood shyness, been related to more depressive symptoms, poor self-esteem,
less favorable attitudes regarding one’s appearance, low life satisfaction, and less
positive affect in adulthood (Kerr, 2000), then depending on when individuals
became shy, the findings in this dissertation might have very different outcomes. For
example, parents might not have been treating children whose shyness was initiated
during middle childhood any different compared with other children. In that case,
our findings might have been stronger for an early onset group. In addition, if
youths were not shy as children, but became so during middle childhood or early
adolescence, they might not have had issues with either peers or parents of the kind
typically connected with shy behaviors. They might have had as many friends as nonshy children, thus making several of the hypotheses in the studies less valid. This
limitation has largely been due to the nature of the datasets used in the dissertation.
Knowing when social fears get established would be a larger endeavor, as we would
need to follow individuals from early childhood until at least adolescence.
Hypothetically speaking, nonetheless, temperamentally shy children might develop
self-conscious shyness later on in life. Even so, not knowing when the participants
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 61
developed shyness might have had an effect on the overall findings in this
dissertation.
There are, conversely, several strengths to the studies in the dissertation. First,
except for Study I, longitudinal data were predominantly used. This has allowed us
to study processes over time, and also assess directions of effects. The nature of the
data thus permitted a closer look on the impact of shyness on friendships, problem
behaviors, and parenting, as well as the other way around. Second, the data used in
all studies were from community-based samples, with high participation rates. Third,
we have dealt with relatively large samples in our studies, which has given us the
statistical power to conduct the comparatively complex tests of data, which might
not have been possible with small numbers of participants. Each study has certain
limitations, respectively. Despite of these limitations, however, the dissertation
provides new insights into the development of youths’ shyness and its influence on
their social worlds.
What IS shyness?
An important issue to return to is the definition of shyness. Partially, this topic still
remains a bit of a puzzle, even though I have learned a lot about shyness, what it is,
and how it affects people’s lives. The puzzle, however, regards the meaning of
shyness. Even though I have consistently decided to stick to a definition of shyness as
wariness in new social encounters, novel places, and unknown people (Asendorpf,
1991; Cheek & Buss, 1981; Cheek & Watson, 1989), and even though I fully
concur with that description, there is still some confusion that remains in my mind.
This puzzlement on my behalf mainly pertains to how one should approach the fact
that shyness, as it has been defined in these studies, might differ from what meaning
people who are not necessarily developmental psychologists might bestow the word.
Or put differently, I often get concerned that some form of psychological imperialism
as suggested by Harris (Harris, 1984) might in fact be at hand in my own studies.
According to Harris’ gloomy observation, we only measure our own idea of shyness
as psychologists, but we choose to call it shyness in any case.
There are, however, some facts that I as a researcher comfort myself with.
First, if shyness indeed were a completely fuzzy or vague concept, it would be difficult
to identify any kinds of tendencies that regard more than a few people at once. In this
dissertation, however, we have shown clear developmental processes that involve
many individuals in chorus. Thus, if shyness as we define it were only present in our
own heads as scientists, it would be improbable that the young people we addressed
in our questionnaires would have made any true sense of the questions we asked
them to tap this behavioral characteristic. We certainly would not have been able to
correlate this measure with other, well-known correlates such as depression
(Elovainio et al., 2004) in a way that made sense to us either. Thus, I believe that our
measure was conceptually good for assessing shyness, and that it made sense to the
participants in our study what we meant when asking them about their social fears.
In addition, and as I have argued before, I would also like to believe that using the
word shyness might make findings such as the ones in this dissertation more easily
accessible to other people. The word shyness brings to mind associations in people’s
62 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
minds. If Harris saw this as something unconstructive, I find that the positive sides to
using the word far outweigh the negative ones. Associations are not necessarily
wrong per se. Without us being able to think about shyness, we would not have been
able to research it in a constructive way either. Making up a new word, as Harris
suggests (Harris, 1984), does not necessarily solve the problem and make the
concept suddenly clear. Instead, I suggest a further clarification of the processes
linked with shyness and its related terms, and the effects these terms altogether have
on people.
Positive aspects of shyness
“I want to resume the life of a shy person”.
Garrison Keillor, author
In addition to being a proponent of the term shyness, I also want to lift up the fairly
neglected positive sides of shyness. From what can be implied from the literature
concerned with the subject, shyness is viewed as something negative. It affects
behaviors, thoughts, and feelings, and can change people’s everyday lives in the most
unhelpful of ways. Yet, one cannot help but think whether there is any value in being
shy? Could shyness be a good thing in certain situations and for some people?
Scholars have argued that shyness must indeed have had some positive function
throughout the human evolution, or it would have not endured as a trait for such a
long time (Carducci, 1999). In fact, situational shyness might serve as means to keep
us in line, and to make us think twice about things to say or do in social interactions
(Carducci, 1999). This, in turn, might prevent us from embarrassing ourselves or
hurting other people’s feelings (Carducci, 1999). If there weren’t any shyness in the
human race, we would live in a world where everyone says exactly what they want to,
and would not care about the consequences of their social actions (Carducci, 1999).
Thus, it seems that shyness to a certain extent or in some situations might fulfill a
positive function in our society by shielding us from social stigma.
In addition, some people might even view shyness as a desirable trait
(Carducci, 1999; Zimbardo, 1977). Several scholars have argued that many features
of shyness could be viewed as valuable (Gough & Thorne, 1986; Leary & Buckley,
2000; Schmidt & Tasker, 2000; Zimbardo, 1977). For example, shy people are
often perceived as modest, self-controlled, and discreet (Leary, Bednarski, Hammon,
& Duncan, 1997). They are also often perceived to be non-impulsive people, who
make up good listeners (Schmidt & Tasker, 2000). Indeed, whatever other negative
perceptions of shy people, they are not perceived as selfish, arrogant, or domineering
(Leary et al., 1997). Shy people might be thought of as showing modest reserve, and
such quiet social styles might often be associated with for example certain celebrities
(Pilkonis & Zimbardo, 1979). Research shows that if shy people themselves report
social fears, doubts about personal worth, and an indecisiveness in dealing with
others as some of their primary facets of behaviors, then others tend to attribute less
positive characteristics to shy people such as timidity and weakness (Gough &
Thorne, 1986). In contrast, if shy individuals’ self-descriptions of shyness are more
inclined towards endurance, self-control, or moderation, then others will view
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 63
shyness in a far more positive light: as reserved, cautious, modest, and self-restrained
(Gough & Thorne, 1986). In this dissertation, it was also shown that shyness might
have developmental significance by protecting young people from getting involved in
problem behaviors at an early age. Thus, there are positive features to shyness that
are easily forgotten in the search of its less positive outcomes.
The developmental significance of shyness in adolescence
“To the timid soul, nothing is possible”.
John Bach, actor
One of the main focal points of this dissertation has been the role of shyness in early
adolescence. Adolescents have been a generally overlooked population in shyness
research. Yet, there are several reasons to believe that shyness should certainly be
considered at this particular stage of development. Among those, as previously
discussed, is the large amount of developmental, emotional, and cognitive transitions
that characterize adolescence. There are hence several ways in how previous research
might not apply to adolescents. Regarding the issue of how shyness impacts
adolescent adjustment, the previous childhood and adult studies cannot fully capture
these processes in a proper manner. Even though both childhood and adulthood are
developmental phases typified by their own problems and characteristics, neither of
the two is comparable with the complexity of for example entering puberty in middle
childhood and early adolescence. The sense of novelty that portrays adolescent
experiences is unique in many ways. Young people are expected to do completely
different things compared to children. They are to assume more responsibilities,
achieve well-functioning social roles, behave as adults, and resemble other grown-ups
in terms of for example following societal norms or expectations. Conversely
however, compared with adults, adolescents are still allowed a great deal of freedom
in the way they behave, in anticipations from others, and other facets of maturity.
Indeed, recent research shows a delayed sense of adolescence in today’s modern
society, termed as emerging adulthood (Arnett, 2004, 2007). Young people are
nowadays allowed to remain and act as adolescents for a great deal longer period of
time, throughout their twenties, compared with earlier generations (Arnett, 2004,
2007). Thus, not only can we not fully compare adolescents with children or adults,
the youth’s roles as young people have also been altered and are constantly modified
in the ever-changing society. Adolescents are thus a distinct group, with specific
features that need to be taken into consideration when studying social fears.
64 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Future directions
“Powdermilk biscuits: Heavens, they’re tasty and expeditious! They’re made from
whole wheat, to give shy persons the strength to get up and do what needs to be
done.”
Garrison Keillor, author
Even though some new aspects of adolescent shyness have been highlighted in this
dissertation, some salient topics have been raised. For example, measuring shyness
can be done in numerous ways. In addition, there are several diverse concepts related
to shyness in some way or another, as was illustrated in the introduction of this
dissertation. Revising the literature and getting a clear picture of the current
knowledge about shyness becomes a challenge once one includes research using
several of the additional terms related to and overlapping with that of shyness, which
in turn can affect the attempts to understand the processes of interest. The question
is how different definitions of shy, reticent behaviors might affect the conclusions
gained from the research results. Even though many of the terms or definitions of
socially fearful behaviors are associated and have common characteristics on a
conceptual level, little is understood about how research participants’ understanding
of the different types of questions in questionnaires and interviews referring to
diverse correlates of shyness influence the findings. That is, will using a shyness scale
compared to a social anxiety scale yield different results, even though the two are
essentially similar? Cross-validating several measures of shy, socially inhibited
behaviors in one and the same study might in future research be one solution to this
pending matter.
Another essential issue to understand is other mechanisms that might be at
hand in the link between adolescent shyness and emotional adjustment. Primarily, it
would be of interest to achieve a clearer insight about how this link is affected by
different social and cultural contexts. On the one hand, socially fearful behaviors
might be perceived differently in dissimilar social contexts and thus have various
consequences for individuals. Shy youths who grow up in other cultures than the
Western ones might be more appreciated as individuals due to their seemingly timid
behaviors. The question is then in which, if any ways, socially fearful behaviors might
be detrimental for emotional adjustment in those cultures. Youths living in the
Western societies might be more subjected to unconstructive aspects of shyness
because their surrounding environment stigmatizes reticent behaviors. In that case,
instead of changing the shy individuals to fit in into the more accepted patterns of
social behaviors, perhaps we should strive for change on societal level by advocating
higher collective tolerance regarding all kinds of unwelcome intricate behavioral
characteristics that are inexorably part of the variety of the human existence. Perhaps
future research might consider strategies of changing the attitudes towards shy
individuals in those who are not shy or socially fearful.
An additional important topic regards adolescent shyness and the
bidirectional nature of the behaviors we have measured in this dissertation. Taking
the example of parents, it is commonly believed that they play the main part in how
their children turn out. In addition, parents have been thought to assert greater
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 65
influence on their children when these are younger, as small children are commonly
under more parental control than adolescents are. As was demonstrated in this
dissertation, however, youths’ characteristics seemed to have more overall influence
on their interactions with parents and peers alike. Even though peers, friends, and
romantic partners start to enter the social worlds of early adolescents in a more
significant manner than before, however, this dissertation shows that parents’
behaviors consequently still have some effect over their children’s behavioral
characteristics. Nonetheless, in order to ascertain just how significant this influence is
over time, a longer time period than yearly measurement over the course of three
years in adolescents’ lives is to be preferred. As actions and reactions of all kinds of
active agents in social relationships change, capturing youths once per year does not
necessarily do these processes justice. Instead, future research might focus on
obtaining information about shy individuals more accurately by increasing the
number of measurement points and measuring these behaviors and feelings more
often.
Finally, a matter worth notice for future research is distinguishing between
participants’ early or late onset of shyness. Having grown up with being shy from
very early childhood, versus developing shyness during middle childhood or early
adolescence almost certainly has different consequences for individuals. It is tempting
to think that shy children who have likely grown up with fewer friends, worse social
relationships, and a social status as the “shy kid”, would be worse off in the long
run. Nonetheless, research does show that shyness developed during early
adolescence has far worse consequences in adulthood than childhood shyness (Kerr,
2000). Besides from a few studies on the subject, not much research attention has
been given to this issue, even though it seems of great importance for our
understanding of social fears. Indeed, if childhood shyness does not affect so many
adult outcomes, should it be a source of concern at all? What are the differences
between how individuals with early- versus late-developing shyness deal with parents,
peers, romantic relationships – in short, their social worlds? Are there any, and if
there are – what are their consequences for these individuals lives? And how likely is
it that shy children might build up self-conscious concerns, similar to those
developed by individuals with late onset shyness? These questions, among other
related ones, remain more or less unexplored for the time being, and should be given
more consideration in future research ventures.
What should be done about shyness?
What advice then could be given to concerned parents, teachers, and significant
people in shy youths’ lives, who worry that the youths’ social fears might be
detrimental for future development? What recommendations might be made to shy
individuals themselves for improving their current conditions? First, there is help to
be had if one needs it. There are as many different therapies attempting to help
problematic shyness as there are therapists, with several types of therapies more
known than others. Second, there are many things one can do by oneself. Googling
the words shyness and book results in 1,350,000 hits on the Internet. Many books
offer different types of coping strategies for shy individuals to improve various
66 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
aspects of their personalities. In addition, countless online self-help sites give advice
to painfully shy individuals. Most importantly, however, shyness should be
“treated” if the individuals themselves perceive it as a problem. That is, if the negative
sides of shyness far outweigh any positive ones, and if the individuals themselves
perceive these to be troublesome and of consequence for their own lives, then shy
individuals should probably be facilitated in some way or another, regardless of
which way they prefer to receive assistance.
In his book Shyness: How Normal Behavior Became a Sickness, Christopher
Lane paints a dismal picture of how social anxiety disorder came to be a disorder in
the first place (Lane, 2007). According to Lane, the socially awkward behaviors
attributed to shyness were historically acknowledged and accepted in earlier times.
These behaviors became a problem, however, when a group of psychiatrists in the US
decided to revise the first version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (Lane, 2007). According to these scholars, social anxiety became nothing
more than an imbalance of chemicals in the brain, which could conveniently be aided
by medicines. The pharmaceutical industry, Lane claims, thrived from this, and
millions of Americans were given pills because they were told that their anxiety and
their social fears were in fact a disease (Lane, 2007). On a similar note, other scholars
have shown concern about drug therapies of shyness, advising parents and
educators not to rush and treat normal personality characteristics in children
(Schmidt & Schulkin, 1999). Indeed, despite devoting an entire book to negative
outcomes of shyness, the authors claim that they in no way want to “pathologize”
shy children – they want to celebrate and not eliminate personality differences
(Schmidt & Schulkin, 1999). In this dissertation, I have arrived at the same
conclusion. Generally speaking, our society places high demands on striving for
uniformity. There are everything from beauty ideals to social ideals that youths feel
they need to live up to, in one way or another. Understandably, however, shyness
should not be seen as merely adding to societal variation if those suffering from it
undeniably perceive it as something negative in their lives. If the shy individuals
themselves do not “suffer” from their predisposition, however, their shyness should
preferably be accepted as a behavioral characteristic that adds to the variety of the
human reality.
Should parents worry at all, then? Whether one believes that shyness is a
nuisance or not, it might appear evident to parents that their children feel bad about
themselves in some way. Indeed, most parents try hard to create the best possibilities
for their children to develop and grow into healthy, happy human beings, who have
confidence in their abilities and can live up to their fullest potentials. In this sense,
having an overly shy child might be worrisome for many parents. The question is
then what advice we should give troubled parents. There are certain things that
parents could think about, both regarding their own reactions to their shy children,
but also about what kinds of behaviors they might value and promote in children.
Recent research on how mothers react to their shy children attempts to answer this
question with a simple message: “Don’t fret, be supportive!” (Coplan, Arbeau, &
Armer, 2008). The study showed that parents of shy children who were warm and
supportive, for example, helped reduce children’s risk of psychosocial maladjustment
linked with shyness during kindergarten years. Even though this research was
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 67
conducted on small children, nonetheless, it does not mean that the same idea would
not be pertinent during adolescence as well. Parents’ warm and supportive behaviors
will not make youths less shy, but they seem to make things better by providing a
secure base for those who tend to feel insecure in unfamiliar social contexts. In
addition, and as research from this dissertation shows, parents might try to refrain
from behaviors that signal coldness or rejection towards their shy children. Instead
of losing too much sleep over their children’s social inabilities or trying to control
them so as to help them be less shy, maybe encouraging them to see the more positive
sides of shyness might increase their confidence and even help them to dare taking
the crucial social leaps that are essential for personal and emotional development.
Closing remarks
“Would anyone dare to say to a woman or a Third World person, “Oh, don’t be a
woman! Oh, don’t be so Third! And yet people make bold with us whenever they
please and put an arm around us and tell us not to be shy.”
Garrison Keillor, author
As persons living in the Western world today, we face many demands and
requirements in order to fit in. We should be confident, wear our hearts on our
sleeves, have many friends, be involved in romantic relationships, and be successful in
our social lives. Even though this is a very stereotypical, perhaps even cynical view of
the modern Western society, this dismal stance is most probably not that far from
the truth. People who lack social skills, show seemingly timid, reticent behaviors,
blush all over when being spoken to or appear as though they have nothing
interesting to say do not fit into this picture. What is worse, others’ might even
assume that these behaviors are something that the person should but cannot
change; they could be deemed as personal inadequacies or proof of meagerness.
Historically seen, it might indeed have been more accepted to be socially fearful. As
the English poet William Wordsworth put it about 150 years ago, “The flower that
smells the sweetest is shy and lowly.” Such views of behaviors, however, might seem
old-fashioned, or outdated these days. But are they? Might it be something wrong
with the judgment passed on shy individuals, instead of the shy individuals’ social
fears? In this dissertation, we did find many detrimental outcomes related to shy
behaviors. But we also found that these behaviors are not isolated events; they are
preceded and followed by reactions and further actions in communication between
shy youths and friends, peers, and parents. Shy youths in this dissertation took
active part in their social worlds, even though they might seem passive in terms of
character to those who do not know or understand them. Though there are no
simple truths nor answers regarding how we should view shyness, one thing is for
certain: shy characteristics and behaviors have always been part of humankind, and
they are here to stay. It is perhaps up to us as self-proclaimed non-shy or not-so-shy
individuals to deal with such presumed social “inadequacies” of others around us.
When we come across people who seem not to want to take part in everything that is
going on, are not good at cracking jokes, seem distant, reticent, or socially awkward
– perhaps our bewilderment about such behaviors says something more about
68 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
ourselves than about the object of our frustration. Perhaps we are too quick to judge
– at first blush.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 69
V Ref er ences
Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., & McFarland, F. C. (2006). Leaders and followers in
adolescent close friendships: Susceptibility to peer influence as a predictor of
risky behavior, friendship instability, and depression. Development and
Psychopathology, 18, 155-172.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
Arkin, R. M., Lake, E. A., & Baumgardner, A. H. (1986). Shyness and selfpresentation. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness:
Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 189-204). New York: Plenum
Press.
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens
through the twenties. New York: Oxford University Press.
Arnett, J. J. (2007). Adolescence and emerging adulthood: A cultural approach (3rd
ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Pearson Education, Inc.
Asendorpf, J. (1989). Shyness as a final common pathway for two different kids of
inhibition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 481-492.
Asendorpf, J. (1990a). Beyond social withdrawal: Shyness, unsociability, and peer
avoidance. Human Development, 33, 250-259.
Asendorpf, J. (1992). A Brunswikean approach to trait continuity: Application to
shyness. Journal of Personality, 60, 54-77.
Asendorpf, J. B. (1987). Videotape reconstruction of emotions and cognitions
related to shyness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 542549.
Asendorpf, J. B. (1990b). Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for
situational specificity and a two-factor model. Developmental Psychology, 26,
721-730.
Asendorpf, J. B. (1990c). The expression of shyness and embarrassment. In W. R.
Crozier (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives from social
psychology (pp. 87-118). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Asendorpf, J. B. (1990d). The expression of shyness and embarrassment. Crozier,
W. Ray.
Asendorpf, J. B. (1991). Development of inhibited children’s coping with
unfamiliarity. Child Development, 62, 1460-1474.
Asendorpf, J. B., & Meier, G. H. (1993). Personality effects on children’s speech in
everyday life: Sociability-mediated exposure and shyness-mediated reactivity
to social situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 10721083.
Asendorpf, J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects on social relationships.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1531-1544.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 71
Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (1999). The natural course of shyness and related
syndromes. In L. A. Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and
social phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and clinical outcomes (pp.
203-223). New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.
Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in
response to noncontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36, 405-417.
Berndt, T. J., & Perry, B. T. (1986). Children’s perceptions of friendships as
supportive relationships. Developmental Psychology, 22, 640-648.
Blöte, A. W., Kint, M. J. W., Miers, A. C., & Westenberg, P. M. (2009). The relation
between public speaking anxiety and social anxiety: A review. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 23, 305-313.
Blöte, A. W., & Westenberg, P. M. (2007). Socially anxious adolescents’ perception
of treatment by classmates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 189-198.
Boivin, M., Hymel, S., & Bukowski, W. M. (1995). The roles of social withdrawal,
peer rejection, and victimization by peers in predicting loneliness and
depressed mood in childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 765785.
Booth-LaForce, C., & Oxford, M. L. (2008). Trajectories of social withdrawal from
grades 1 to 6: Prediction from early parenting, attachment, and temperament.
Developmental Psychology, 44, 1298-1313.
Borkovec, T. D., Fleischmann, D. J., & Caputo, J. A. (1973). The measurement of
anxiety in an analogue social situation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 41, 157-161.
Breidenstein-Cutspec, P., & Goering, E. (1989). Exploring cultural diversity: A
network analysis of the communicative correlates of shyness within the black
culture. Communication Research Reports, 6, 37-46.
Briggs, S. R. (1985). A trait account of social shyness. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Self,
situations, and social behavior (pp. 35-64). Beverly Hills: SAGE.
Briggs, S. R. (1988). Shyness: Introversion or neuroticism? Journal of Research in
Personality, 22, 290-307.
Briggs, S. R., & Smith, T. G. (1986). The measurement of shyness. In S. R. Briggs, J.
M. Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and
treatment (pp. 47-60). New York: Plenum Press.
Brown, B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott
(Eds.), At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent (pp. 171-196).
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Brown, B. B., Clasen, D. R., & Eicher, S. A. (1986). Perceptions of peer pressure, peer
conformity dispositions, and self-reported behavior among adolescents.
Developmental Psychology, 22, 521-530.
Brown, B. B., Lohr, M. J., & Trujillo, C. (1990). Multiple crowds and multiple life
styles: adolescents’ perceptions of peer-group stereotypes. In R. E. Muus (Ed.),
Adolescent behavior and society: A book of readings (Vol. 4, pp. 30-36). NY:
McGraw-Hill.
72 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Bruch, M. A. (1989). Familial and developmental antecedents of social phobia: Issues
and findings. Clinical Psychology Review. Special Issue: Social phobia, 9, 3747.
Bruch, M. A., Giordano, S., & Pearl, L. (1986). Differences between fearful and selfconscious shy subtypes in background and current adjustment. Journal of
Research in Personality, 20, 172-186.
Bruch, M. A., Gorsky, J. M., Collins, T. M., & Berger, P. A. (1989). Shyness and
sociability reexamined: A multicomponent analysis. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 57, 904-915.
Bruch, M. A., Heimberg, R. G., Harvey, C., McCann, M., Mahone, M., & Slavkin, S.
L. (1992). Shyness, alcohol expectancies, and alcohol use: Discovery of a
suppressor effect. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 137-149.
Bruch, M. A., Rivet, K. M., Heimberg, R. G., Hunt, A., & McIntosh, B. (1999).
Shyness and sociotropy: Additive and interactive relations in predicting
interpersonal concerns. Journal of Personality, 67, 373-406.
Bruch, M. A., Rivet, K. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Levin, M. A. (1997). Shyness, alcohol
expectancies, and drinking behavior: Replication and extension of a
suppressor effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 193-200.
Buchanan, C. M., Eccles, J. S., & Becker, J. B. (1992). Are adolescents the victims of
raging hormones? Evidence for activational effects of hormones on moods
and behavior at adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 62-107.
Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal competence, and
adjustment during preadolescence and adolescence. Child Development, 61,
1101-1111.
Bukowski, W. M., Gauze, C., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (1991). Differences and
consistency between same-sex and other-sex peer relationships during early
adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 29, 255-263.
Burgess, K. B., Wojslawowicz, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Rose-Krasnor, L., & BoothLaForce, C. (2006). Social information processing and coping strategies of
shy/withdrawn and aggressive children: Does friendship matter? Child
Development, 77, 371-383.
Burke, R. S., & Stevens, R. S. (1999). Social anxiety and drinking in college students:
A social cognitive theory analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 513-530.
Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco, California:
Freeman.
Buss, A. H. (1986). A theory of shyness. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J. Warren
(Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 39-46). New
York: Plenum Press.
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carducci, B. J. (1999). Shyness: A bold new approach. New York: HarperCollins
Publisher, Inc.
Carducci, B. J. (2000). What shy individuals do to cope with their shyness: A content
analysis. In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, Consolidation and
Change (pp. 171-185). London: Routledge.
Carducci, B. J., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1995). Are you shy? Psychology Today, 34-40.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 73
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1988). Moving away from the world: Lifecourse patterns of shy children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 824-831.
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age three predict
personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth
cohort. Child Development, 66, 486-498.
Cattell, R. B. (1973). Personality and mood by questionnaire. Oxford, England:
Jossey Bass.
Cheek, J. M., & Briggs, S. R. (1990). Shyness as a personality trait. In W. R. Crozier
(Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives from social psychology (pp.
315-337). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cheek, J. M., & Busch, C. M. (1981). The influence of shyness on loneliness in a new
situation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 572-577.
Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 41, 330-339.
Cheek, J. M., Carpentieri, A. M., Smith, T. G., Rierdan, J., & Koff, E. (1986).
Adolescent shyness. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.),
Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 105-116). New York:
Plenum Press.
Cheek, J. M., & Krasnoperova, E. N. (1999). Varieties of shyness in adolescence and
adulthood. In L. A. Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and
social phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and clinical outcomes (pp.
224-250). New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.
Cheek, J. M., & Melchior, L. A. (1990). Shyness, self-esteem, and self-consciousness.
In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of social and evaluation anxiety (pp. 4782). New York: Plenum Press.
Cheek, J. M., Melchior, L. A., & Carpentieri, A. M. (1986). Shyness and self-concept.
In L. M. Hartman & K. R. Blankstein (Eds.), Advances in the study of
communication and affect (pp. 113-131). New York: Plenum Press.
Cheek, J. M., & Watson, A. K. (1989). The definition of shyness: Psychological
imperialism or construct validity. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,
4, 85-95.
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998).
Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian
toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677-686.
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, B. (1995). Social and school adjustment of shy and
aggressive children in China. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 337-349.
Comrey, A. L. (1965). Scales for measuring compulsion, hostility, neuroticism, and
shyness. Psychological Reports, 16, 697-700.
Conger, J. J. (1956). Reinforcement theory and the dynamics of alcoholism.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 17, 296-305.
Coplan, R. J., Arbeau, K. A., & Armer, M. (2008). Don’t fret, be supportive!
Maternal characteristics linking child shyness to psychosocial and school
adjustment in kindergarten. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 359371.
74 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Coplan, R. J., Coleman, B., & Rubin, K. H. (1998). Shyness and little boy blue: Iris
pigmentation, gender, and social wariness in preschoolers. Developmental
Psychobiology, 32, 37-44.
Coplan, R. J., Gavinski-Molina, M.-H., Lagacé-Séguin, D. G., & Wichmann, C.
(2001). When girls versus boys play alone: Nonsocial play and adjustment in
kindergarten. Developmental Psychology, 37, 464-474.
Coplan, R. J., Prakash, K., O’Neil, K., & Armer, M. (2004). Do you "want" to play?
Distinguishing between conflicted shyness and social disinterest in early
childhood. Developmental Psychology, 40, 244-258.
Coplan, R. J., Rubin, K. H., Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Stewart, S. L. (1994). Being
alone, playing alone, and acting alone: Distinguishing among reticence and
passive and active solitude in young children. Child Development, 65, 129137.
Crawford, L. A., & Novak, K. B. (2004). Reactivity to conspicuousness and alcohol
use among college students: The moderating effect of alcohol expectancies.
Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1845-1849.
Crozier, R. (1979). Shyness as anxious self-preoccupation. Psychological Reports,
44, 959-962.
Crozier, W. R. (1981). Shyness and self-esteem. British Journal of Social Psychology,
20, 220-222.
Crozier, W. R. (1990). Social psychological perspectives on shyness, embarrassment,
and shame. In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives
from social psychology (pp. 19-58). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Crozier, W. R. (1995). Shyness and self-esteem in middle childhood. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 65, 85-95.
Crozier, W. R. (1999). Individual differences in childhood shyness: Distinguishing
fearful and self-conscious shyness. In L. A. Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.),
Extreme fear, shyness, and social phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and
clinical outcomes Oxford University Press, Inc. (pp. 14-29). New York:
Oxford University Press, Inc.
Crozier, W. R. (2000). Shyness and social relationships: Continuity and change. In
W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, Consolidation and Change (pp.
1-21). London: Routledge.
Crozier, W. R. (2001). Understanding shyness: Psychological perspectives. New
York: Palgrave.
Dadds, M. R., & Barrett, P. M. (2001). Practitioner review: Psychological
management of anxiety disorders in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 42, 999-1011.
Damon, W. (1983). Social and personality development. New York: Norton.
Dell’Osso, L., Rucci, P., Ducci, F., Ciapparelli, A., Vivarelli, L., Carlini, M.,
Ramacciotti, C., & Cassano, G. B. (2003). Social anxiety spectrum. European
Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 253, 286-291.
Dodge, C. S., Heimberg, R. G., Nyman, D., & O’Brien, G. T. (1987). Daily
heterosocial interactions of high and low socially anxious college students: A
diary study. Behavior Therapy, 18, 90-96.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 75
Dubow, E. F., Boxer, P., & Huesmann, L. R. (2008). Childhood and adolescent
predictors of early and middle adulthood alcohol use and problem drinking:
The Columbia County Longitudinal Study. Addiction, 103, 36-47.
Eggleston, A. M., Woolaway-Bickel, K., & Schmidt, N. B. (2004). Social anxiety and
alcohol use: Evaluation of the moderating and mediating effects of alcohol
expectancies. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, 33-49.
Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., Puttonen, S., Heponiemi, T., Pulkki, L., & KeltikangasJärvinen, L. (2004). Temperament and depressive symptoms: A populationbased longitudinal study on Cloninger’s psychobiological temperament
model. Journal of Affective Disorders, 83, 227-232.
Evans, M. A. (1993). Communicative competence as a dimension of shyness. In K.
H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness
in childhood (pp. 189-212). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1969). Personality structure and measurement.
London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., & Ghera, M. M.
(2005). Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology and behavior within a
developmental framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 235-262.
Fox, N. A., Sobel, A., Calkins, S., & Cole, P. (1996). Inhibited children talk about
themselves: Self-reflection on personality development and change in 7-year
olds. In M. Lewis & M. W. Sullivan (Eds.), Emotional Development in
Atypical Children (pp. 131-147). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Publishers.
Fuligni, A. J., Eccles, J. S., Barber, B. L., & Clements, P. (2001). Early adolescent peer
orientation and adjustment during high school. Developmental Psychology,
37, 28-36.
Furmark, T., Tillfors, M., Everz, P. O., Marteinsdottir, I., Gefvert, O., & Fredrikson,
M. (1999). Social phobia in the general population: Prevalence and
sociodemographic profile. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34,
416-424.
Furnham, A., & Henderson, M. (1983). The mote in thy brother’s eye, and the beam
in thine own: Predicting one’s own and others’ personality test scores. British
Journal of Psychology, 74, 381-389.
Galambos, N. L., Kolaric, G. C., Sears, H. A., & Maggs, J. L. (1999). Adolescents’
subjective age: An indicator of perceived maturity. Journal of Research on
Adolescence, 9, 309-337.
Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young
children. Child Development, 55, 1005-1019.
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference,
and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental
study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625-635.
Gazelle, H., & Ladd, G. W. (2003). Anxious solitude and peer exclusion: A diathesisstress model of internalizing trajectories in childhood. Child Development, 74,
257-278.
76 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Gest, S. D. (1997). Behavioral inhibition: Stability and associations with adaptation
from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 72, 467-475.
Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2003). Childhood peer relationships: Social
acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School Psychology, 41,
235-284.
Goering, E., & Breidenstein-Cutspec, P. (1989). The web of shyness: A network
analysis of communicative correlates. Communication Research Reports, 6,
111-118.
Goering, E., & Breidenstein-Cutspec, P. (1990). The co-cultural experience of
shyness: A comparison of the friendship networks of black communicators
and white communicators. The Howard Journal of Communications, 2, 262275.
Gortmaker, S. L., Kagan, J., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1997). Daylength during
pregnancy and shyness in children: Results from Northern and Southern
hemispheres. Developmental Psychobiology, 31, 107-114.
Gough, H. G., & Thorne, A. (1986). Positive, negative, and balanced shyness: Selfdefinitions and the reactions of others. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J.
Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 205226). New York: Plenum Press.
Gren-Landell, M., Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., Bohlin, G., Andersson, G., & Svedin, C.
G. (2009). Social phobia in Swedish adolescents. Social Psychiatry and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44, 1-7.
Grüner, K., Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The relationship between anxious
rearing behaviours and anxiety disorders symptomatology in normal
children. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 30, 2735.
Güroglu, B., Van Lieshout, C. F. M., Haselager, G. J. T., & Scholte, R. H. J. (2007).
Similarity and complementarity of behavioral profiles of friendship types and
types of friends: Friendships and psychosocial adjustment. Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 17, 357-386.
Ham, L. S., Bonin, M., & Hope, D. A. (2007). The role of drinking motives in social
anxiety and alcohol use. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 991-1003.
Ham, L. S., & Hope, D. A. (2005). Incorporating social anxiety into a model of
college student problematic drinking. Addictive Behaviors, 30, 127-150.
Harris, P. R. (1984). Shyness and psychological imperialism: On the dangers of
ignoring the ordinary language roots of the terms we deal with. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 169-181.
Hartman, L. M. (1986). Social anxiety, problem drinking, and self-awareness. New
York: Plenum Press.
Haselager, G. J., Hartup, W. W., Van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M.
A. (1998). Similarities between friends and nonfriends in middle childhood.
Child Development, 69, 1198-1208.
Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., & DeRose, L. (2005). Links among gender, inhibition,
and parental socialization in the development of prosocial behavior. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 51, 467-493.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 77
Heiser, N. A., Turner, S. M., & Beidel, D. C. (2003). Shyness: Relationship to social
phobia and other psychiatric disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41,
209-221.
Himadi, W. G., Arkowitz, H., Hinton, R., & Perl, J. (1980). Minimal dating and its
relationship to other social problems and general adjustment. Behavior
Therapy, 11, 345-352.
Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2001). Parent-child interactions and anxiety
disorders: An observational study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39,
1411-1427.
Iannotti, R. J., Bush, P. J., & Weinfurt, K. P. (1996). Perceptions of friends’ use of
alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana use among urban school children: A
longitudinal analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 21, 615-632.
Insko, C. A., & Wilson, M. (1977). Interpersonal attraction as a function of social
interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 903-911.
Jackson, T., Towson, S., & Narduzzi, K. (1997). Predictors of shyness: A test of
variables associated with self-presentational models. Social Behavior and
Personality, 25, 149-154.
Jessor, R. (1992). Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for
understanding and action. Developmental Review, 12, 374-390.
Jones, W. H., Briggs, S. R., & Smith, T. (1986). Shyness: Conceptualization and
measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 629-639.
Jones, W. H., & Carpenter, B. N. (1986). Shyness, social behavior, and relationships.
In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on
research and treatment (pp. 227-238). New York: Plenum Press.
Jones, W. H., Freemon, J. E., & Goswick, R. A. (1981). The persistence of loneliness:
Self and other determinants. Journal of Personality, 49, 27-48.
Jones, W. H., & Russell, D. (1982). The Social Reticence Scale: An objective
instrument to measure shyness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 629631.
Kagan, J. (1999). The concept of behavioral inhibition. In L. A. Schmidt & J.
Schulkin (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and social phobia: Origins, biological
mechanisms, and clinical outcomes (pp. 3-13). New York: Oxford University
Press, Inc.
Kagan, J. (2000). Inhibited and uninhibited temperaments: Recent developments. In
W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, consolidation and change (pp.
22-29). London: Routledge.
Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1986). Shyness and temperament. In S. R. Briggs, J. M.
Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment
(pp. 81-90). New York: Plenum Press.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and uninhibited types of
children. Child Development, 60, 838-845.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of
behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459-1473.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Snidman, N., Gibbons, J., & Johnson, M. O. (1988).
Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar.
Child Development, 59, 1580-1589.
78 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Kagan, J., Reznick, S. J., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of childhood
shyness. Science, 240, 117-256.
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Temperamental factors in human development.
American Psychologist, 46, 856-862.
Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Julia-Sellers, M., & Johnson, M. O. (1991). Temperament
and allergic symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 332-340.
Kagan, J., Snidman, N., McManis, M., Woodward, S., & Hardway, C. (2002). One
measure, one meaning: Multiple measures, clearer meaning. Development and
Psychopathology. Special Issue: Multiple levels of analysis, 14, 463-475.
Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent
friendships. The American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427-436.
Kemple, K. M. (1995). Shyness and self-esteem in early childhood. Journal of
Humanistic Education & Development, 33, 173-183.
Kerr, M. (2000). Childhood and adolescent shyness in long-term perspective: Does it
matter? In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, Consolidation and
Change (pp. 64-87). London: Routledge.
Kerr, M. (2001). Culture as a context for temperament: Suggestions from the life
courses of shy Swedes and Americans. In T. D. Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm
(Eds.), Temperament in context (pp. 139-152). New Jersey: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Kerr, M., Lambert, W. W., & Bem, D. J. (1996). Life-course sequelae of childhood
shyness in Sweden: Comparison with the United States. Developmental
Psychology, 32, 1100-1105.
Kerr, M., Lambert, W. W., Stattin, H., & Klackenberg-Larsson, I. (1994). Stability of
inhibition in a Swedish longitudinal sample. Child Development, 65, 138-146.
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and several
forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of
monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 36, 366-380.
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2003). Parenting of adolescents: Action or reaction? In A. C.
Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics: The
neglected side of family relationships (pp. 121-151). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Kiesner, J. (2007). Peers and problem behavior: Have we
missed something? In R. C. M. E. Engels (Ed.), Friends, lovers and groups:
Key relationships in adolescence. Hot topics in developmental research (pp.
125-153). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Kerr, M., Tremblay, R. E., Pagani, L., & Vitaro, F. (1997). Boy’s behavioral
inhibition and the risk of later delinquency. Archives of General Psychiatry,
54, 809-816.
Kidorf, M., & Lang, A. R. (1999). Effects of social anxiety and alcohol expectancies
on stress-induced drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 134-142.
Kim, S., Brody, G. H., & Murry, V. M. (2003). Factor structure of the early
adolescent temperament questionnaire and measurement invariance across
gender. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 268-294.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 79
Kinney, D. A. (1993). From Nerds to Normals: The recovery of identity among
adolescents from middle school to high school. Sociology of Education, 66,
21-40.
Kuczynski, L. (2003). Beyond bidirectionality: Bilateral conceptual frameworks for
understanding dynamics in parent-child relations. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.),
Handbook of Dynamics in Parent-Child Relations (pp. 1-24). Thousand
Oaks CA: Sage.
Kuczynski, L., & Parkin, C. M. (2006). Agency and bidirectionality in socialization:
Interactions, transactions, and relational dialectics. In J. E. Grusec & P.
Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 259283). New York, NY Guilford Press.
La Greca, A., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with
peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26,
83-94.
LaBrie, J., Pedersen, E. R., Neighbors, C., & Hummer, J. F. (2008). The role of selfconsciousness in the experience of alcohol-related consequences among college
students. Addictive Behaviors, 33, 812-820.
Lane, C. (2007). Shyness: How normal behavior became a sickness. London: Yale
University Press.
Lawrence, B., & Bennett, S. (1992). Shyness and education: The relationship between
shyness, social class and personality variables in adolescents. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 62, 257-263.
Lazarus, P. J. (1982). Incidence of shyness in elementary-school age children.
Psychological Reports, 51, 904-906.
Leary, M. R. (1986). Affective and behavioral components of shyness: Implications
for theory, measurement, and research. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J.
Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 27-38).
New York: Plenum Press.
Leary, M. R., Bednarski, R., Hammon, D., & Duncan, T. (1997). Blowhards, snobs,
and narcissists: Interpersonal reactions to excessive egotism. In R. M.
Kowalski (Ed.), Aversive interpersonal behaviors (pp. 111-131). New York:
Plenum.
Leary, M. R., & Buckley, K. E. (2000). Shyness and the pursuit of social acceptance.
In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, Consolidation and Change
(pp. 139-153). London: Routledge.
Leary, M. R., & Dobbins, S. E. (1983). Social anxiety, sexual behavior, and
contraceptive use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 13471354.
Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1993). The Interaction Anxiousness Scale:
Construct and criterion-related validity. Journal of Personality Assessment,
61, 136-146.
Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1995). Social anxiety. New York: The Guilford
Press.
Leary, M. R., Kowalski, R. M., & Campbell, C. D. (1988). Self-presentational
concerns and social anxiety: The role of generalized impression expectancies.
Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 308-321.
80 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Lemerise, E. A. (1997). Patterns of peer acceptance, social status, and social
reputation in mixed-age preschool and primary classrooms. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 43, 199-218.
Lewinsky, H. (1941). The nature of shyness. British Journal of Psychology, 32, 105113.
Lewis, B. A., & O’Neill, H. K. (2000). Alcohol expectancies and social deficits relating
to problem drinking among college students. Addictive Behaviors, 25, 295299.
Mandel, N. M., & Shrauger, J. S. (1980). The effects of self-evaluative statements on
heterosocial approach in shy and nonshy males. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 4, 369-381.
Marshall, J. R., & Lipsett, S. (1994). Social phobia: From shyness to stage fright.
New York: Basic Books.
Masia, C. L., & Morris, T. L. (1998). Parental factors associated with social anxiety:
Methodological limitations and suggestions for integrated behavioral
research. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 211-228.
Miller, R. S. (1986). Embarrassment: Causes and consequences. In S. R. Briggs, J. M.
Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment
(pp. 295-311). New York: Plenum Press.
Miller, R. S. (1995). On the nature of embarrassability: Shyness, social evaluation,
and social skill. Journal of Personality, 63, 315-337.
Mills, R. S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (1998). Are behavioural and psychological control
both differentially associated with childhood aggression and social
withdrawal? Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 30, 132-136.
Moehler, E., Kagan, J., Oelkers-Ax, R., Brunner, R., Poutska, L., Haffner, J., &
Resch, F. (2008). Infant predictors of behavioural inhibition. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 26, 145-150.
Morry, M. M. (2005). Relationship satisfaction as a predictor of similarity ratings: A
test of the attraction-similarity hypothesis. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 22, 561-584.
Mosier, C. I. (1937). A factor analysis of certain neurotic tendencies. Psychometrika,
2, 263-287.
Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Perceived parental rearing behaviour and
anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children. Personality and Individual
Differences, 25, 1199-1206.
Natale, M., Entin, E., & Jaffe, J. (1979). Vocal interruptions in dyadic
communication as a function of speech and social anxiety. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 865-878.
Neto, F. (1992). Loneliness among Portuguese adolescents. Social Behavior and
Personality, 20, 15-22.
Neto, F. (1993). The Satisfaction With Life Scale: Psychometrics properties in an
adolescent sample. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 125-134.
O’Hare, T. M. (1990). Alcohol expectancies and social anxiety in male and female
undergraduates. Addictive Behaviors, 15, 561-566.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 81
Oh, W., Rubin, K. H., Bowker, J. C., Booth-LaForce, C., Rose-Krasnor, L., &
Laursen, B. (2008). Trajectories of social withdrawal from middle childhood
to early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 553-566.
Page, R. M. (1990). Shyness and sociability: A dangerous combination for illicit
substance use in adolescent males? Adolescence, 25, 803-806.
Papini, D. R., & Roggman, L. A. (1992). Adolescent perceived attachment to parents
in relation to competence, depression, and anxiety: A longitudinal study.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 12, 420-440.
Park, A., Sher, K. J., & Krull, J. L. (2006). Individual differences in the "Greek effect"
on risky drinking: The role of self-consciousness. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 20, 85-90.
Paulsen, E., Bru, E., & Murberg, T. A. (2006). Passive students in junior high school:
The associations with shyness, perceived competence and social support.
Social Psychology of Education, 9, 67-81.
Pearson, V. (1991). Western theory, Eastern practice: Social group work in Hong
Kong. Social Work with Groups, 14, 45-58.
Phillips, G. M. (1997). Reticence: A perspective on social withdrawal. In J. A. Daly &
J. C. McCroskey (Eds.), Avoiding communication: Shyness, reticence, and
communication apprehension (pp. 129-229). New Jersey: Hampton Press,
Inc.
Pilkonis, P. A. (1977a). Shyness, public and private, and its relationship to other
measures of social behavior. Journal of Personality, 45, 585-595.
Pilkonis, P. A. (1977b). The behavioral consequences of shyness. Journal of
Personality, 45, 596-611.
Pilkonis, P. A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). The personal and social dynamics of
shyness. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Emotions, personality, and psychotherapy (pp.
133-160). New York: Plenum.
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1986). Genetics and shyness. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek
& H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp.
63-80). New York: Plenum Press.
Pozo, C., Carver, C. S., Wellens, A. R., & Scheier, M. F. (1991). Social anxiety and
social perception: Construing others’ reactions to the self. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 355-362.
Prinstein, M. J., Borelli, J. L., Cheah, C. S. L., Simon, V. A., & Aikins, J. W. (2005).
Adolescent girls’ interpersonal vulnerability to depressive symptoms: A
longitudinal examination of reassurance-seeking and peer relationships.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Special Issue: Toward a Dimensionally
Based Taxonomy of Psychopathology, 114, 676-688.
Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Peer crowd affiliation and internalizing
distress in childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal follow-back study.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 325-351.
Prisbell, M. (1991). Shyness and self-reported competence. Communication Research
Reports, 8, 141-148.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in
the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401.
82 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in the development of
anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 47-67.
Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalized anxiety. In M. W. Vasey & M.
M. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. (481–
503). New York: Oxford University Press.
Rapee, R. M., & Spence, S. H. (2004). The etiology of social phobia: Empirical
evidence and an initial model. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 737-767.
Rogosch, F., Chassin, L., & Sher, K. J. (1990). Personality variables as mediators
and moderators of family history risk for alcoholism: Conceptual and
methodological issues. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 310-318.
Rohsenow, D. J. (1983). Drinking habits and expectancies about alcohol’s effects for
self versus others. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 752756.
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child
Development, 73, 1830-1843.
Rosenberg, A., & Kagan, J. (1987). Iris pigmentation and behavioral inhibition.
Developmental Psychobiology, 20, 377-392.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic books.
Rubin, K. H. (1993). The Waterloo longitudinal project: Correlates and
consequences of social withdrawal from childhood to adolescence. In K. H.
Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in
childhood (pp. 291-314). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Publishers.
Rubin, K. H., & Asendorpf, J. B. (1993). Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness
in childhood: Conceptual and definitional issues. In K. H. Rubin & J. B.
Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood
(pp. 3-17). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D. (2002). Stability and socialbehavioral consequences of toddlers’ inhibited temperament and parenting
behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483-495.
Rubin, K. H., Cheah, C. S. L., & Fox, N. (2001). Emotion regulation, parenting and
display of social reticence in preschoolers. Early Education & Development,
12, 97-115.
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L. (1990). Maternal beliefs about adaptive and
maladaptive social behaviors in normal, aggressive, and withdrawn
preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 419-435.
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L. (1991). Conceptualizing developmental pathways to
internalizing disorders in childhood. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
Science, 23, 300-317.
Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). The transaction
between parents’ perceptions of their children’s shyness and their parenting
styles. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 937-957.
Rubin, K. H., Stewart, S. L., & Chen, X. (1995). Parents of aggressive and
withdrawn children. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting:
Children and parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 255-284). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Publishers.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 83
Rubin, K. H., Wojslawowicz, J. C., Rose-Krasnor, L., Booth-LaForce, C., & Burgess,
K. B. (2006). The best friendships of shy/withdrawn children: Prevalence,
stability, and relationship quality. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34,
143-157.
Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., & Demaray, M. K. (2008). Gender differences in the
relationship between perceived social support and student adjustment during
early adolescence. School Psychology Quarterly, 23, 496-514.
Russell, D., Cutrona, C. E., & Jones, W. H. (1986). A trait-situational analysis of
shyness. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.), Shyness:
Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 239-249). New York: Plenum
Press.
Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human
Development, 18, 65-79.
Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A
conceptualization model. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 641-669.
Schmidt, L. A., & Fox, N. A. (1994). Patterns of cortical electrophysiology and
autonomic activity in adults’ shyness and sociability. Biological Psychology,
38, 183-198.
Schmidt, L. A., & Fox, N. A. (1995). Individual differences in young adults’ shyness
and sociability: Personality and health correlates. Personality & Individual
Differences, 19, 455-462.
Schmidt, L. A., & Fox, N. A. (1999). Conceptual, biological, and behavioral
distinctions among different categories of shy children. In L. A. Schmidt & J.
Schulkin (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and social phobia: Origins, biological
mechanisms, and clinical outcomes (pp. 47-66). New York: Oxford
University Press, Inc.
Schmidt, L. A., & Robinson, T. N. (1992). Low self-esteem in differentiating fearful
and self-conscious forms of shyness. Psychological Reports, 70, 255-257.
Schmidt, L. A., & Schulkin, J. (1999). Epilogue. In L. A. Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.),
Extreme fear, shyness, and social phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and
clinical outcomes. New York: Oxford University Press.
Schmidt, L. A., & Tasker, S. L. (2000). Childhood shyness: Determinants,
development and ‘depathology’. In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness:
Development, Consolidation and Change (pp. 30-46). London: Routledge.
Schneider, B. H. (1999). A multimethod exploration of the friendships of children
considered socially withdrawn by their school peers Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 27, 115-123.
Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an
outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1008-1015.
Seifer, R., Sameroff, A. J., Barrett, L. C., & Krafchuk, E. (1994). Infant temperament
measured by multiple observations and mother report. Child Development,
65, 1478-1490.
Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Correspondence
among informants on parenting: Preschool children, mothers, and observers.
Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 53-68.
84 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Shenkar, O., & Ronen, S. (1987). The cultural context of negotiations: The
implications of Chinese interpersonal norms. Journal of Applied Behavioral
Science, 23, 263-275.
Simmons, R. G., Rosenberg, F., & Rosenberg, M. (1973). Disturbance in the selfimage at adolescence. American Sociological Review, 38, 553-568.
Siqueland, L., Kendall, P. C., & Steinberg, L. (1996). Anxiety in children: Perceived
family environments and observed family interaction. Journal of Clinical
Child Psychology, 25, 225-237.
Smith, H. M., & Betz, N. E. (2002). An examination of efficacy and esteem pathways
to depression in young adulthood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49,
438-448.
Snijders, T. A. B., Steglich, C. E. G., Schweinberger, M., & Huisman, M. (2007).
Manual for SIENA version 3. Groningen: University of Groningen.
Snyder, C. R., & Smith, T. W. (1986). On being “shy like a fox”: A selfhandicapping analysis. In S. R. Briggs, J. M. Cheek & H. J. Warren (Eds.),
Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 161-172). New York:
Plenum Press.
Stemberger, R. T., Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Calhoun, K. S. (1995). Social
phobia: An analysis of possible developmental factors. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 104, 526-531.
Stone, M., & Brown, B. B. (1998). In the eye of the beholder: Adolescents’
perceptions of peer crowd stereotypes. In R. E. Muus (Ed.), Adolescent
behavior and society: A book of readings (pp. 158-169). NY: McGraw-Hill.
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt,
and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70, 1256-1269.
Tran, G. Q., Haaga, D. A. F., & Chambless, D. L. (1997). Expecting that alcohol use
will reduce social anxiety moderates the relation between social anxiety and
alcohol consumption. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 535-553.
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Townsley, R. M. (1990). Social phobia: Relationship
to shyness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 497-505.
Twentyman, C. T., & McFall, R. M. (1975). Behavioral training of social skills in shy
males. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 384-395.
Udry, J. R., Kovenock, J., Morris, N. M., & Van den Berg, B. J. (1995). Childhood
precursors of age at first intercourse for females. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
24, 329-337.
Urberg, K. A. (1992). Locus of peer influence: Social crowd and best friend. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 439-450.
Van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., & Oakman, J. M. (1998). The relationships of
behavioral inhibition and shyness to anxiety disorder. The Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 186, 425-431.
Van Brakel, A. M. L., Muris, P., Bögels, S. M., & Thomassen, C. (2006). A
multifactorial model for the etiology of anxiety in non-clinical adolescents:
Main and interactive effects of behavioral inhibition, attachment, and parental
rearing. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 15, 569-579.
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
I 85
Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448-457.
Vazsonyi, A. T., Trejos-Castillo, E., & Huang, L. (2006). Risky sexual behaviors,
alcohol use, and drug use: A comparison of eastern and western European
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, e1-e11.
Whaley, S. E., Pinto, A., & Sigman, M. (1999). Characterizing interactions between
anxious mothers and their children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 67, 826-836.
Williams, J. M., & Dunlop, L. C. (1999). Pubertal timing and self-reported
delinquency among male adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 157-171.
Wood, J. J., McLeod, B. D., Sigman, M., Hwang, W. C., & Chu, B. C. (2003).
Parenting and childhood anxiety: Theory, empirical findings, and future
directions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 134-151.
Zimbardo, P. G. (1977). Shyness: What it is, what to do about it. Reading,
Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley.
86 I
NEJRA BEŠIĆ
At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’ Social Worlds
Publications in series Örebro Studies in Psychology
1. Andershed, Henrik, Antisocial Behavior in Adolescence – The Role of
Individual Characteristics. 2002.
2. Trost, Kari, A new look at parenting during adolescence: Reciprocal
interactions in everyday life. 2002.
3. Jensen, Eva, (Mis)understanding and Learning of Feedback Relations in a
Simple Dynamic System. 2004.
4. Wester Herber, Misse, Talking to me? – Risk Communication to a
diverse Public. 2004.
5. Boersma, Katja, Fear and avoidance in the development of a persistent
musculoskele tal pain problem. Implications for secondary prevention. 2005.
6. Jansson, Markus, Insomnia: Psychological Mechanisms and Early Intervention. A Cognitive-Behavioral Perspective. 2005.
7. Lindblom, Karin, Utbrändhet i normalbefolkningen. Arbets- och individfaktorers relation i utvecklingen mot eller återhämtning från utbrändhet. 2006.
(Vetenskaplig uppsats)
8. Almqvist, Lena, Children’s Health and Developmental Delay: Positive
Functioning in Every-day Life. 2006.
9. Johansson, Peter, Understanding psychopathy through the study of longterm violent offenders. 2006.
10. Persson, Andreas, Leisure in Adolescence: Youth’s activity choices and why
they are linked to problems for some and not others. 2006.
11. Ojala, Maria, Hope and worry: Exploring young people’s values, emotions,
and behavior regarding global environmental problems. 2007.
12. larsson, Mats, Human Iris Characteristics as Biomarkers for Personality.
2007.
13. pakalniskiene, Vilmante, Harsh or Inept Parenting, Youth Characteristics
and Later Adjustment. 2008.
14. skoog, therése, On the developmental significance of female pubertal
timing. 2008.
15. brav, Agneta, Industrial Work Groups. The Impact of Job Design, Leader
Support and Group Processes on Initiative and Self-organization. 2008.
´ nejra, At First Blush: The Impact of Shyness on Early Adolescents’
16. beŠic,
Social Worlds. 2009.
Was this manual useful for you? yes no
Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the work of artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Download PDF

advertisement